Evolution of Female Polymorphism in a Neotropical Radiation of Lizards
by
Evi Achil Dominique Paemelaere
A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty of
Auburn University
in partial fulfillment of the
requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
Auburn, Alabama
August 9, 2010
Keywords: Anoles, macro-evolution, micro-evolution,
dorsal patterns, habitat, predation
Copyright 2010 by Evi Achil Dominique Paemelaere
Approved by
F. Stephen Dobson, Chair, Professor of Biological Sciences
Craig Guyer, Professor of Biological Sciences
Michael Wooten, Professor of Biological Sciences
Bertram Zinner, Associate Professor of Mathematics and Statistics
ii
Abstract
Color pattern polymorphism ? the occurrence of multiple color patterns within
populations ? has provided excellent opportunities for the study of evolution. Recently, a
renewed interest in polymorphism was sparked by the realization that variation among
females may be more important to evolutionary processes than originally thought. Few
species have been thoroughly studied with regards to female polymorphism (FP), and
most studies have focused on single species, thus ignoring the broad effects of
evolutionary history. I present anoles (Squamata: Polychrotidae) as a model for studying
FP. This speciose lizard radiation contains several species with discontinuous variation in
female dorsal patterns.
My overall question addressed the origination and maintenance of FP in anoles.
As recommended for studies of possible adaptive traits, both pattern and process of its
evolution were addressed by combining phylogenetic and geographic analyses with
population studies of the female polymorphic anole Norops humilis. Phylogentic signal
was determined to be moderate, resulting from independent evolution of FP in ancestors
of multiple anole clades as well as in individual species, as indicated by parsimony and
maximum likelihood methods. Combining phylogenetic and geographic distribution of
FP showed a dichotomy in the evolution of this trait. Among the basal radiation of
anoles, island species were commonly polymorphic, while mainland species were not.
The opposite pattern was seen in the rest of the anole radiation. Comparative analyses
iii
revealed similarities in habitat use and especially perch use among female polymorphic
species, indicating that FP may have evolved in response to selective pressures typical in
those environments. Based on these results and because the dorsal patterns appear cryptic
against their background, I examined the commonly accepted but virtually untested
hypothesis that this polymorphic trait evolved in response to predator pressure.
Population level studies in Norops humilis examined predation on clay models, habitat
selection, and survival of different female morphs in juveniles and adults. Possible
predator associated mechanisms include frequency dependent predation (FDP) and
morph specific microhabitat choice to reduce visibility to predators. Similar survival rates
of morphs refuted FDP. Although a predation experiment indicated morph specific
variation in predation depending on perch type, females were not found to choose perches
in accordance with the lowest predation rate. I conclude that female dorsal patterns in
Norops humilis at La Selva are not maintained by predation alone and I suggest
alternative hypotheses.
iv
Acknowledgments
First and foremost, I would like to express my gratitude to my advisor, Dr. F.
Stephen Dobson for his support throughout my Doctoral program and suggestions for
improvement to my dissertation. His guidance helped me prepare for my future
endeavors as a professional scientist. I would also like to recognize the members of my
Advisory Committee, Drs. Craig Guyer, Michael Wooten and Bertram Zinner for their
constructive criticism of my ideas and assistance with manuscripts. A special thank you
goes to Dr. Craig Guyer for sharing his experience and his data. I extend special thanks to
Drs. James B. Grand, Scott R. Santos and Todd D. Steury for their statistical advise.
I thank all my lab mates and colleagues in Biological Sciences for interesting
discussions on research ideas. Helpful comments and suggestions have been provided by
Jennifer Deitlof, David Lorenzio, John Steffen and Linda Pastorello. For assistance in the
field and with data entry, I thank Anna Liner, Ryan Conway and Brittany Way. I would
also like to thank the staff of La Selva Biological Station for their assistance with
logistics.
I owe my deepest gratitude to my parents, Michel and Maryl?ne Paemelaere-
Piron, for their continued support in all of my endeavors. I would also like to thank my
family and friends for their encouragement, companionship and advise. This dissertation
was supported by funding from the Auburn University Graduate School, a Fellowship
from the Organization for Tropical Studies, and a research grant from Elkin Amaya.
v
Table of Contents
Abstract............................................................................................................................... ii
Acknowledgments ............................................................................................................. iv
List of Tables .................................................................................................................... vii
List of Figures.................................................................................................................. viii
Chapter One: An Introduction to Female Polymorphism
A brief history of polymorphism .............................................................................1
Female polymorphism ? a further challenge to our understanding of evolution ....3
Anoles ......................................................................................................................9
Chapter Two: The Evolution of Female Polymorphism: A Vertebrate Model
Introduction ..........................................................................................................11
Methods ................................................................................................................15
Results ..................................................................................................................19
Discussion ............................................................................................................24
Chapter Three: Dorsal Pattern Polymorphism and Habitat Use in Female Anoles
(Squamata: Polychrotidae)
Introduction ..........................................................................................................39
Methods ................................................................................................................42
Results ..................................................................................................................47
Discussion ............................................................................................................48
vi
Chapter Four: Survival of Alternative Dorsal Pattern Morphs in Females of the Anole
Norops humilis.
Introduction ..........................................................................................................60
Methods ................................................................................................................62
Results ..................................................................................................................65
Discussion ............................................................................................................66
Chapter Five: Predation on Females with Alternative Dorsal Patterns in Norops humilis.
Introduction ..........................................................................................................73
Methods ................................................................................................................76
Results ..................................................................................................................79
Discussion ............................................................................................................80
Chapter Six: Female Dorsal Pattern Polymorphism in Norops humilis (Squamata:
Polychrotidae): Perch Use and a Test of the Microhabitat Hypothesis.
Introduction ..........................................................................................................89
Methods ................................................................................................................92
Results ..................................................................................................................94
Discussion ............................................................................................................96
Summary and Conclusions ...........................................................................................106
Literature Cited ..............................................................................................................112
vii
List of Tables
Chapter Two, Table 1: Nomenclature applied for clades within anoles............................33
Chapter Three, Table 1: Model selection steps to determine the relationship between
macro and micro-habitat variables and the presence of female polymorphism.................55
Chapter Four, Table 1: Goodness of fit tests for saturated model examining effects of
morph on survival of juvenile and adult females of Norops humilis. ...............................71
Chapter Four, Table 2: Modeling recapture (P) and survival (?) as a function of age (a)
and dorsal pattern morph (m).............................................................................................72
Chapter Six, Table 1: Model selection steps....................................................................102
Chapter Six, Table 2: Average perch heights ..................................................................103
Chapter Six, Table 3: Model selection steps without leaf litter data ...............................104
Chapter Six, Table 4: Average perch heights and standard deviations for 1982-1983
dataset without leaf litter observations ............................................................................105
viii
List of Figures
Chapter Two, Figure 1: Evolution of female polymorphism (FPP) in Anolis...................34
Chapter Two, Figure 2: Geographic distribution of female polymorphism in anoles ......38
Chapter Three, Figure 1: Phylogeny with patterns and habitat variables related to
distribution of female polymorphism in anoles .................................................................56
Chapter Three, Figure 2: Distribution of female polymorphism among habitat types......59
Chapter Four: Figure 1: Frequencies of female morphs observed.....................................70
Chapter Five, Figure 1: Clay models .................................................................................85
Chapter Five, Figure 2: Bite marks categorized as predation............................................86
Chapter Five, Figure 3: Predation frequencies for pattern-background combinations from
experiment 1.......................................................................................................................87
Chapter Five, Figure 4: Predation for different heights and diameters of woody stems
from experiment 2..............................................................................................................88
Chapter Six, Figure 1: Female morphs occurring in Norops humilis in the population of
La Selva Biological Station, Costa Rica..........................................................................101
CHAPTER ONE
AN INTRODUCTION TO FEMALE POLYMORPHISM
?If you come to any more conclusions about polymorphism, I shd be very glad
to hear the result: it is delightful to have many points fermenting in one?s brain?
Darwin, C. R. to Hooker, J. D., [25 Feb 1846]
Much of modern evolutionary theory arose from the early interest in phenotypic
polymorphism, as pointed out by Darwin (1871). Polymorphism is the occurrence of
distinct phenotypes within one species. More specifically, it is the discontinuous variation
between individuals of the same developmental stage within a population (Clark 1976;
Huxley 1955). Although some of this variation can result from environmental effects,
focus here is solely on genetically determined polymorphism (Clark 1976; Ford 1940).
Evolutionary theory predicts that polymorphism should be rare, because selection is
expected to favor one optimal morph and even without selection, drift would eventually
lead to the fixation of one allele (Futuyma 1998). But, genetic polymorphism was found
to be surprisingly abundant in natural populations (Harris 1966; Lewontin and Hubby
1966; Nevo 1978), thus challenging evolutionary theory.
A brief history of polymorphism
Much of our knowledge on origination and maintenance of phenotypic
polymorphism finds its roots in population genetics. Theoretical models were developed
to explain the occurrence of stable polymorphism. The concept of heterozygote
advantage (heterosis) formed the first adaptive explanation, stating that polymorphism
1
could be maintained as long as the fitness of the heterozygote was higher than that
of either homozygote (Fisher 1922; Fisher 1930; Haldane 1955; Jones 1917; Rendel
1953). Within a homogenous environment, this would indeed be the only selective
mechanism to maintain allelic variation within the population. Levene (1953), however,
showed that polymorphism can also be maintained in heterogeneous environments
without heterozygote advantage. Stable polymorphism was also shown to be possible
under directional change in selective pressures (Haldane and Jayakar 1962; later revised
by Cornette 1981). Thus, attention was drawn to selective pressures varying in space and
time (Dempster 1955; Gillespie 1973; Gillespie and Langley 1974; Levins and
MacArthur 1966). Later on, models also explored effects of changes in fitness of
genotypes based on their relative frequency (Lewontin 1958; Wright 1948), resulting in
soft selection (Wallace 1975). This was later refined for phenotypes, rather than
genotypes (Clarke and O'Donald 1964). Wright (1931) stimulated thought on the
importance of chance variation (mutation and drift) in maintaining polymorphism, and
formed the inspiration for Kimura?s work on neutral processes (Kimura 1983; Kimura
1991). Neutral and selective processes need not be mutually exclusive, and both can
contribute to origination and maintenance of polymorphism (Craze 2009; Dobzhansky
and Pavlovsky 1957; Wright 1948). The discussion is thus reduced to how much each
mechanism contributes to the maintenance of polymorphism.
The study of color pattern polymorphism facilitated testing of these models and
much work focused on the variation of colors and patterns on the shells of Cepaea land
snails (Cain and Sheppard 1954a; Jones et al. 1977; Lamotte 1952). Selective pressures
for color patterns are attractiveness to mates, thermoregulatory capacity and avoidance of
2
predation (Brodie 1992; Endler 1995; Forsman 1997; Forsman and Shine 1995; Gibson
and Falls 1979; Watt 1968), and color pattern polymorphism can persist in cases of
spatial or temporal variation in these selective pressures (Hedrick 1986; Hedrick 1990;
Schmidt and Rand 2001). A more specific form of fluctuating selective pressure for
dorsal color patterns is frequency-dependent selection by predators (Ayala and Campbell
1974; Endler and Greenwood 1988). Initially introduced by Poulton (1884), the concept
of frequency dependent predation was never explicitly addressed in early population
genetic models on polymorphism. Mate choice too can be frequency dependent and thus
maintain polymorphism (Ayala 1972). Other evolutionary mechanisms could be
heterozygote advantage (Hedrick 1999), correlational selection (Brodie 1989; Brodie
1992; Forsman and Appelqvist 1998) or genetic drift (Hoffman et al. 2006).
Female polymorphism ? a further challenge to our understanding of evolution
Although color polymorphism may occur in males and females alike, as in the
popular examples of Cepaea snails, or peppered moths (Cain and Sheppard 1954a;
Kettlewell 1955), variation in color pattern may also be limited to one gender. Many
species show gender-based variation in color patterns or other ornaments ? particularly in
males but also in some females ? and this is often ascribed to variation in quality and
related mate choice (Amundsen and Forsgren 2001; Amundsen et al. 1997; Gross 1996;
Roulin 2004). Most of this variation, however, is continuous (Hill 2006).
As with continuous variation, gender-based polymorphic traits have mostly been
studied in males, and are often linked to alternative mating strategies. For example, in
bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) some males resemble females in color pattern
and behavior, allowing them to fertilize eggs without building nests or defending
3
territories like the other males (Dominey 1980; Gross and Charnov 1980). A well-known
example is the side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana), where the three throat color
morphs represent different degrees of territoriality and mate guarding in what is often
referred to as a ?rock-paper-scissor? game (Sinervo and Lively 1996; Zamudio and
Sinervo 2000). Similarly, the dark and white males of the lek-breeding ruff (Philomachus
pugnax) represent territorial and sneaker courtship tactics (Lank et al. 1995), and a third,
uncommon morph mimics females (Jukema and Piersma 2006).
For females, however, such alternative mating strategies are not expected,
because males are generally the showy sex, while females are choosy (Bateman 1948;
Darwin 1871; Trivers 1972). Moreover, female coloration patterns were long thought to
be merely a byproduct of selection acting on males (Fisher 1930; Lande 1980; Lande
1987). It is therefore not surprising that female variation in color patterns has largely
been ignored (Amundsen 2000a; Amundsen 2000b; Andersson 1994). In the last two
decades, however, a renewed interest in females was sparked by the realization that
variation among females within a population may be more important to evolutionary
processes than originally thought (Amundsen and P?rn 2006; Svensson et al. 2009).
Female polymorphism has been documented for a variety of species, but only a few have
been subject to research on the possible evolutionary significance of its occurrence (see
below). In spite of some sparse examples of extensive research, female polymorphism
continues to challenge evolutionary biologist to test hypotheses that could explain the
commonness of female polymorphism in some radiations of species. Here, I review
examples of female polymorphism to provide a basis for current knowledge on this topic.
4
Invertebrates
One of the first studied occurrences of female polymorphism was in butterflies.
The families Papilionidae, Nymphalidae, Pieridae and Acraeidae all have species for
which female-limited polymorphism has been reported (Owen 1971; Wickler 1968). The
explanation for the color pattern variation in female color patterns in butterflies was
ascribed to the mimicking of various unpalatable species or of different morphs within
those species as a protection against predation (Fisher and Ford 1929; Joron and Mallet
1998; Owen 1971; Wickler 1968). Male-like patterns may occur in some females, as is
the case in Papilio dardanus antinorii (Wickler 1968). Genetically, the process resulting
in female polymorphism in mimetic butterflies may be regulated by modifier genes
linked to the female-specific chromosome or be controlled by autosomal genes (Clarke
and Sheppard 1962; Clarke and Sheppard 1963; Sheppard 1967; Smith 1975). Female
mate choice appears to restrict development of polymorphic males (Krebs and West
1988), but differential habitat use may also be important in the limitation of
polymorphism to females (Joron 2005). Among moths, another interesting example of
female polymorphism is provided by the British pyralid moth, Acentropus niveus (Huxley
1955). Females of this moth are either a normal, male-like form or they can be an aquatic
and flightless form that exploits an ecological niche outside of the reach of normal
morphs.
Within Odonata (dragonflies and damselflies) various species have been
described as female polymorphic (Tillyard 1917), and several have been subject to
extensive studies. Although some variation exists among species, generally two or three
female morphs can be distinguished, and one of these color morphs tends to resemble the
5
male coloration pattern (Robertson 1985; Tillyard 1917). More specifically, when male-
like color patterns are the dominant form in females, these tend to be bright colors, while
if the female-specific color patterns occur in higher frequency than the male-like form,
colors tend to be dull (Tillyard 1917). In damselflies, female polymorphism is coded by
serial dominance of alleles in an autosomal gene (Cordero 1990; Johnson 1964; Johnson
1966). Maintenance of female-limited polymorphism in damselflies has been explained
by a balance between sexual conflict and predation, with support for frequency dependent
and possibly temperature dependent variation (Bots et al. 2009; Cordero et al. 1998;
Robertson 1985), but no hypothesis has succeeded in fully explaining it.
Fiddler crabs too contain at least a few species with female polymorphism. The
often stunning coloration patterns in fiddler crabs vary within and across species. Colors
may change in an individual throughout the ontogenetic cycle or as a more immediate
change in response to the environment (Crane 1975). Generally, females vary more in
color pattern than males and therefore, several species of fiddler crabs have been
described with female polymorphism in color patterns. Color patterns may assist in
individual recognition, and specifically in mate recognition by males (Detto et al. 2008).
In jumping spiders (Salticidae) of the genus Phiale, discovery of female color
polymorphism led to a revision of the genus (Galiano 1981), but its significance remains
unstudied (Oxford and Gillespie 1998).
Diving beetles (Coleoptera: Dytiscidae) display a very different variation in
morphology; here, females vary in dorsal structures used during mating (Bergsten et al.
2001). Similarly, females of the copepod Paroithona pacifica (Cyclopoida: Oithonidae)
vary in some exoskeleton characteristics, such as the number of spines on their swimming
6
legs (Ferrari and Bottger 1986). Neither polymorphism is well understood. Another
example of structural variation in morphology of females is the African bat bug
Afrocimex constrictus (Cimicidae) (Reinhardt et al. 2007). Female bat bugs differ in
genital structures, apparently to reduce the number of traumatic copulations which are
common in this and related species.
Vertebrates
Female polymorphs in vertebrates has been documented mainly for lizards, but
amphibians and even birds include examples as well. The toad Bufo periglenes, now
extinct, was an exceptionally bright colored species of its genus, and females showed
some variation in color and in presence or absence of a vertebral pale stripe (Savage
1966). In the warbler Wilsonia citrina, males and females perch in slightly different
microhabitats, but some females develop the melanistic male coloration as they mature
and use a male-like microhabitat (Lynch et al. 1985). This female melanism develops as
the individual matures (Rappole and Warner 1980), which could explain the somewhat
continuous variation in the trait. Plumage color polymorphism also appears in adult
females of some cuckoos where some are ?barred red? and some ?unbarred grey?,
supposedly as a mimic of the sparrow hawk (Voipio 1951; fide Huxley 1955).
A well-studied case for vertebrates is the side-blotched lizard Uta stansburiana
(Phrynosomatidae). Mostly known for its polymorphism in males, the side-blotched
lizard is now studied for the dual polymorphism in females: throat color and dorsal
pattern. Alternative throat colors in adult females are associated with egg and clutch size
(Sinervo et al. 2000). Throat color is regulated by a single locus with an apparent
hierarchical dominance between alleles (Sinervo and Zamudio 2001; Sinervo et al. 2000).
7
Dorsal pattern was found to be regulated by interactions between yolk oestradiol levels
and the social environment (Lancaster et al. 2007). The combination of throat color and
dorsal pattern results in a complex system where benefits of dorsal pattern alone are
difficult to study (Lancaster et al. 2007).
Some examples with only one female color pattern polymorphism can also be
found in lizards. In the common lizard Lacerta vivipara (Lacertidae) different colored
bellies of females reflect alternative reproductive strategies and determines aggression
and avoidance behavior between females (Vercken and Clobert 2008; Vercken et al.
2007). Among several Australian scincid lizards of the genera Saprocinus and
Lampropholis, dorsal patterns of females differ in the absence or presence of a white
mid-lateral stripe (Forsman and Shine 1995; Sadlier et al. 1993). In Lampropholis
delicata (Scincidae) this may be related to variation in predation pressure, especially of
gravid females, between populations and combined with gene flow (Forsman and Shine
1995). In some species, like the striped plateau lizard Sceloporus virgatus
(Phrynosomatidae) throat color female-biased polymorphism is seasonal, in which case it
can be linked to seasonal activities such as signaling reproductive status (Smith 1946;
Weiss 2002).
This review is by no means exhaustive, but points out a few remarkable features
of female polymorphism: 1) invertebrates have been subjected to more thorough studies
and fewer examples are available among vertebrates, 2) much emphasis has been placed
on conspicuous coloration, which is likely the result of observer bias for such color
patterns, 3) probably as a result of this focus on conspicuous color patterns, female
polymorphism is often studied in light of mate choice or reproductive strategies
8
(Svensson et al. 2009), while other mechanisms could maintain polymorphism in
females. In this dissertation, I highlight the occurrence of a common, non-conspicuous
variant of female polymorphism in anoles.
Anoles
Anoles (Squamata: Polychrotidae) form a diverse, species rich and widespread
group of Neotropical lizards. Female dorsal pattern polymorphism (FPP) has been
described for several species of anoles (e.g. Savage 2002; Schoener and Schoener 1976).
Anoles have been thoroughly studied and they have proven to be good model organisms
for behavioral, evolutionary and ecological questions (Losos 1994). Hence, much
information on their biology is available for comparative studies. The occurrence of FPP
in species of anoles, even though understudied, is generally known and pattern
descriptions are often included in species descriptions. Anoles could thus serve as a
model for the study of FPP, and an understanding of the occurrence of FPP in anoles is an
essential addition to current knowledge on these organisms. The overall goal of my
dissertation research is to better understand the evolution of FPP in anoles.
Evolution of a polymorphic trait has rarely been studied in a phylogenetic context
(Jose et al. 2008), even though knowledge of the evolutionary history is important in the
interpretation of population based studies and comparative analyses (Cheverud et al.
1985; Felsenstein 1985). Although population based approaches may give insight in
short-term mechanisms of maintenance, phylogenetic approaches address long-term
evolutionary patterns among species and indicate whether traits are novel or ancestral
(Harvey and Pagel 1991). A common critique to using phylogeny in the study of a
possibly adaptive character is the misunderstanding that the apomorphic character (novel
9
trait) alone is sufficient to infer adaptation (Coddington 1990). A recent review on the use
of phylogeny in testing adaptation clarified different viewpoints and concluded that
evolution should be studied at different levels, while recognizing limitations and
advantages of each (Grandcolas and D'Haese 2003). The authors suggested three criteria
for adaptation: the genetic, the phylogenetic and the selection criterion, and as such
emphasized the need for studying traits in the context of both clades and populations. The
selection criterion supplements the phylogenetic criterion; a common selective
environment has to be demonstrated for species sharing the trait of interest (e.g. Harvey
and Pagel 1991).
Considering the sparse knowledge of female polymorphism in anoles, and no
understanding of its genetic basis, an appropriate approach is thus to combine
phylogenetic and population-based levels of study. In chapter 2, I focus on the
evolutionary pattern of FPP in anoles, revealing the occurrence of FPP across species in
light of their phylogenetic history and geographic distribution. Chapter 3 investigates
whether female polymorphic anoles share a common environment that could exert
selective pressures that favor the evolutionary maintenance of FPP. The next three
chapters address micro-evolutionary patterns of FPP in a population of a common
mainland anole, Norops humilis, studying two of the major mechanisms proposed for
maintaining color pattern polymorphism: frequency dependent predation and predation in
heterogeneous habitats.
10
CHAPTER TWO
THE EVOLUTION OF FEMALE POLYMORPHISM: A VERTEBRATE MODEL
INTRODUCTION
The incidence of multiple morphs within a population ? polymorphism ? has
inspired theories of evolution since early explorations into evolutionary biology (Darwin
1859; Darwin 1871). Variation in morphological characters such as size or color between
males and females (sexual dimorphism) and among males led to sexual selection theory
(Darwin 1871), and both have been thoroughly investigated in a wide variety of
organisms (e.g., Andersson and Iwasa 1996; Houde 1997; Pradhan and Van Schaik 2009;
Shine 1979; Shuster and Wade 2003; Sinervo and Lively 1996). Research on
polymorphism among females has concentrated mostly on insects, but has generally
remained understudied. In recent years, the number of attempts to better understand
female polymorphism has noticeably increased (Stamps and Gon 1983; Svensson et al.
2009).
Female polymorphism has been documented mostly for species of Lepidoptera
(butterflies) and Odonata, (damselflies and dragonflies) and rarely in vertebrates (Fisher
and Ford 1929; Richards 1961; Tillyard 1917; Wickler 1968). Although polymorphism
has been described for other species, only a few of these have been subjected to research
on its occurrence. Studies indicate that multiple selective pressures may interact to
11
maintain polymorphism limited to females (e.g., Joron and Mallet 1998; Sirot et al.
2003). Female color morphs in damselflies, for example, are thought to exert alternative
techniques to balance predation and male harassment (Bots et al. 2009; Cordero et al.
1998; Robertson 1985). In spite of intensive field studies and molecular research,
especially in Odonata, female polymorphism remains controversial (Andres et al. 2000;
Svensson et al. 2009).
Surprisingly, most research has focused on single species. Although such
contributions are valuable for our understanding of how female polymorphism is
maintained, they largely ignore the question of origination. In contrast, macro-
evolutionary approaches pose species? characteristics in light of their evolutionary history
and consider the possibility that current characteristics may have resulted from historical
events, rather than as current adaptations (Dobson 1985). When a character is shared
between sister taxa, the most parsimonious explanation is that the trait was retained after
origination in their common ancestor. Thus, the trait may have evolved in an ancestral
environment, which may differ from the current environment (Dobson 1985).
Conversely, independent evolution of a trait in distantly related species suggests an
adaptive character of the trait if it coincides with occupation of similar environments
(Harvey and Pagel 1991; Larson and Losos 1996; Schluter 2000). In this case, the shared
character can have evolved from different ancestral states (convergent evolution), or from
the same ancestral state (parallel evolution) (Harvey and Pagel 1991; Zhang and Kumar
1997). Phylogenetic approaches thus provide direction for future research and indicate
possible limitation of population based studies (Grandcolas and D'Haese 2003).
12
Analogous to effects of phylogenetic relationships, geographic distribution can
promote or constrain evolutionary change. Wiens et al. (2009) emphasized the
importance of combining phylogenetic with biogeographical analyses. Geographic
isolation is thought to stimulate convergent evolution when the different locations contain
similar environments (Simpson 1953). Furthermore, an environment could become
saturated with species sharing a common trait. In this case, geographic isolation could
further stimulate parallel evolution through release from this saturated environment into a
similar, non-saturated one, where the same trait can then evolve. The end result is that
more species could share a particular adaptation (Wiens et al. 2009). A combined
phylogenetic and geographical analysis of a recurring trait results in the evolutionary
perspective required before further examination of the processes resulting in the
evolution and maintenance of a trait.
A study on the phylogenetic context of female polymorphism in damselflies
suggested that it was the ancestral state for two genera (Van Gossum and Mattern 2008).
In this case, the reason for origination and for maintenance of the trait may each need
different explanations and both are important to our understanding of polymorphism. In
Papilio swallowtail butterflies, however, female polymorphism is a derived character that
evolved repeatedly (Kunte 2009). Such macro-evolutionary studies require thoroughly
studied lineages for which phylogenetic relationships are well-known. For female
polymorphism, particularly in vertebrates, such lineages have rarely been described. Yet,
they could serve as model systems.
Anoles (Squamata: Polychrotidae) are a good model system for female
polymorphism, because female variation in their dorsal patterns is easily observable, and
13
patterns are known to be heritable (Calsbeek et al. 2008). Females of polymorphic
species generally show two or three variations in dorsal patterns within a population.
Savage (2002) summarized female dorsal patterns into five distinct morphs: vertebral
light stripe with or without dark border, diamonds, dark chevrons, and dark X marks. The
patterns are already clearly expressed in juveniles and are consistent throughout life. The
same patterns are found across species. With nearly 400 species and recent advances in
research of their phylogenetic history anoles lend themselves well to macro-evolutionary
approaches to studying female polymorphism. Furthermore, anoles are an excellent
model system for the study of ecology, evolution and behavior (e.g. Beuttell and Losos
1999; Losos 1992a; Losos 1994). The presence of female polymorphism in anoles,
however, has received surprisingly little attention, although its presence is commonly
known (e.g. Fitch 1975; Savage 2002; Stamps and Gon 1983). Two studies found a
relationship between morph and perch use (Schoener and Schoener 1976; Steffen 2009).
A third study detected density effects in immuno-competence variation between morphs
(Calsbeek et al. 2008). Differences between morphs found thus far fail to explain the
evolutionary origin and the maintenance of female polymorphism.
Because anoles are a good model for the study of female polymorphism, we
examined the evolution of the presence of female dorsal pattern polymorphism (FPP)
across species. Particularly, we tested whether FPP is a derived character and has evolved
independently among anoles. Furthermore, we incorporated a geographical analysis to
investigate whether geographic isolation could have contributed to the current
distribution of FPP among species.
14
METHODS
Data
Species descriptions were used to determine the presence or absence of pattern
polymorphism in females (Avila-Pires 1995; Campbell 1998 ; Dixon and Soini 1986;
Duellman 1978; Duellman 2005; Fitch 1975; Garrido and Hedges 2001; Lazell 1972; Lee
1996; Lee 2000; Nicholson et al. 2001; Rivero 1998; Savage 2002; Schwartz and
Henderson 1991; Stafford and Meyer 2000; Stejneger 1900; van Buurt 2005; Vitt and de
la Torre 1996). This allowed only species level assessments, so that species were scored
positive for female polymorphism, even if not all populations showed this. Presence or
absence of dorsal pattern polymorphism was based on the vertebral zone alone.
Geographical variation, subspecies variation and variation due to metachromatism (i.e.
color change) were not considered as polymorphism for the purpose of this study.
Nomenclature
There are nearly 400 species of anoles and, in spite of extensive research on their
evolutionary history, some phylogenetic relationships and the related nomenclature
remain ambiguous. Most anoline lizards were placed in the genus Anolis, but the great
diversity led to a division into groups and series (first by Etheridge 1960; formalized by
Williams 1976), only some of which are monophyletic in recent phylogenies. Some
species that were originally considered to belong to separate anoline genera
(Chamaelinorops, Phenacosaurus and Chamaeleolis) were later found to have arisen
from within Anolis (Hass et al. 1993; Poe 1998). A division of Anolis into multiple
genera was proposed (Guyer and Savage 1986), but this remains controversial
15
(Cannatella and de Queiroz 1989; Guyer and Savage 1992; Williams 1989). Only the
name Norops is regularly used as a genus name to refer to Etheridge?s (1960) Beta
section (See Nicholson 2002 and references therein), but recognition of this genus
renders a group with the remaining anoles paraphyletic. Recent phylogenies, however,
indicate that there are several rather well-established clades within anoles, even though
the relationship between some of these major clades still remains somewhat unclear
(Jackman et al. 1999; Nicholson et al. 2005; Poe 2004), and a more practical
nomenclature system may result. Because in our study the use of one genus name for so
many species is impractical and many clades are rather stable entities, we will refer to
monophyletic clades and series within clades (Table 1). We will still use Anolis as the
genus name to avoid any confusion with future names that are likely to arise now
relationships among anoles are being resolved.
Phylogeny
No single phylogeny for all anole species was available. Therefore, we combined
recent phylogenies, based on Nicholson et al. (2005), with species added from Poe
(2004). The latter phylogeny was also used to resolve remaining polytomies. Species
synonyms were checked using the Reptile Database (JCVI). The major differences
between the phylogenies we used was the placement of the cybotes series and placement
of some species within the mainland beta anoles. None of the variation between the
phylogenies, however, affected our major conclusions.
16
Character evolution and phylogenetic signal
To study character evolution, we applied two different methods: parsimony and
maximum likelihood (ML). The benefits and critiques on these techniques were reviewed
in detail by Cunningham et al. (1998) and Cunningham (1999). Parsimony reconstruction
fails to recognize rapid evolution of a trait, and parsimony is not reliable for unequal rates
of evolution between loss and gain of a trait. These issues are largely overcome by
maximum likelihood methods (Schluter et al. 1997). A drawback of ML is its dependence
on branch lengths, which may differ depending on which of several standard
transformations are applied. These transformation are used when accurate estimates of
branch lengths are lacking. Interestingly, ML does not necessarily favor the most
parsimonious path of character evolution, although when the number of character
changes is limited, parsimony and ML methods generally result in similar reconstructions
(Pagel 1999).
Because maximum likelihood approaches (see below) are sensitive to branch
length, this analysis was run with equal branch lengths as well as Grafen?s (1989)
arbitrary transformation of branch lengths. The major conclusions, however, remained
unaltered, regardless of the branch length transformation applied. Considering the slightly
more conservative approach of equal branch lengths to missing taxa and previous
evolutionary studies on anoles applying this transformation (Ord and Martins 2006; Poe
et al. 2007), only results for equal branch lengths are presented.
Unordered parsimony analysis was performed in Mesquite (Maddison and
Maddison 2008). This assumes equal rates of forward and backward evolution.
Maximum likelihood estimates were obtained with the ?geiger? package in R Version
17
2.8.1 (Harmon et al. 2008). First, we compared an equal-rates (ER) model of evolution,
as used in parsimony, with an all-rates-different (ARD) model and found that the ARD
model fit the data slightly better (-Log Likelihood
(ER)
= -87.47, -Log Likelihood
(ARD)
= -
85.07, P = 0.0286, AIC
(ER)
= 177, AIC
(ARD)
= 174). Particularly, forward evolution was
found to be roughly two times faster than backward evolution (Gain: 0.227 + 0.05, Loss:
0.097 + 0.02). Maximum likelihood values for the ARD model were plotted onto the
phylogenetic tree for further analysis.
Finally, we tested for phylogenetic signal using Pagel?s lambda (Pagel 1999).
This compares the distribution of a trait among taxa between a star phylogeny (? = 0, i.e.
all phylogenetic structure removed) and a given phylogenetic structure (? = 1, i.e. all
branch lengths maintained). Lambda thus varies between zero and one, and a higher
value indicates a stronger covariance between the phylogeny and the distribution of the
trait. To determine if lambda is significantly different from zero, a maximum likelihood
ratio test was used in R (Harmon et al. 2008; Yang 2006).
Geographic analysis
For the geographic analysis, all island species were considered, even when
phylogenetic relationships were unresolved. Because many mainland species remain
poorly known, only the species for which detailed descriptions were available were
included for the mainland. To assess geographic distribution of female polymorphism in
anoles, we used species distribution maps and presence/absence data of FPP at the
species level. This ignores the possible absence of FPP in certain locations of a species?
distribution. To test hypotheses related to the distribution of FPP, a more in-depth study
of presence or absence of FPP per population will be required. Species distributions were
18
included in the phylogeny to combine a geographic and phylogenetic analysis. Some
species, however, were not included in any available phylogeny and could only be
incorporated for the geographical analysis.
RESULTS
For 180 species of anoles a detailed description of dorsal pattern was found. Of
these, over 50 species were described as having female dorsal pattern polymorphism
(FPP). The majority of the species with female polymorphism were mainland species,
even though island species constituted the larger part in the dataset. Only 162 of these
species were included in the phylogenies we used for our analysis.
Phylogenetic signal
We found a significant phylogenetic signal in the presence of FPP among anoles
(Log Likelihood phylogeny = -81.89, Log Likelihood unstructured = -90.61, P < 0.0001).
The lambda value under the ARD model was 0.644. Because lambda varies between zero
and one, our value indicates a moderately strong phylogenetic signal.
Character evolution
Parsimony Analysis
Parsimony analysis indicated that the ancestral state was absence of FPP (Figure
1.D.). For the 162 species included, the evolutionary pattern of FPP required 28 steps.
The entire carolinensis clade lacked FPP (Figure 1.C). Three other members of this group
for which more detailed phylogenetic relationships were not known also lacked FPP. All
other clades had at least one species with FPP. Overall, parsimony analysis indicated the
independent evolution of FPP across clades, but FPP was also found in closely related
19
species, signifying that FPP likely arose in their ancestor. This was the case for island
dactyloa minus A. bonairensis (Figure 1.D). In the remaining groups that had multiple
species with FPP, including mainland norops, cybotes and the ctenonotus clades,
alternative hypotheses on number of gains versus losers were possible, based on
parsimony.
Within the norops clade, few data were available for species endemic to Mexico,
which form the basal radiation for mainland norops. Therefore, FPP could have arisen in
an ancestor of all mainland species or in the ancestor of the mainland species ranging
south of Mexico (Figure 1.A.). In either case, the trait was lost in A. lionotus, A.
notopholis, A. townsendi, A. lineatus and the ancestor of the petersii series, in which a
reversal to FPP appeared in A. woodi. An alternative scenario described evolution of FPP
after the split of the petersii series. In this case, the ancestor of A. nitens and its closely
related species evolved FPP separately from the rest of the mainland species.
In the cybotes clade, FPP may have developed independently in A. whitemani, A.
armouri and A. cybotes, or alternatively in an ancestor of these species, with losses in A.
shrevei and A. haetianus (Figure 1.B). Within the cristatellus series of the ctenonotus
clade, the alternatives were evolution of FPP independently in A. cristatellus and A.
ernestwilliamsi, or in the ancestor of these with a reversal in A. desechensis (Figure 1.B.).
All species of the bimaculatus series could have evolved FPP independently, with the
exception of the ancestor of A. marmoratus and A. sabanus. There were three, equally
parsimonious scenarios, with FPP evolving earlier in the clade and subsequent loss of this
trait (Figure 1.B.).
20
Maximum Likelihood Estimates
The maximum likelihood analysis supported the majority of the parsimony
analysis (Figure 1.A.- D.). Importantly, the ancestor of all anoles only had a 0.003 %
likelihood of FPP. Where the parsimony analysis indicated that FPP and no FPP in an
ancestor were equally likely, the maximum likelihood parsimony analysis generally
resulted in values near 50% likelihood. Similarly, presence and absence of FPP in
ancestors based on parsimony mostly resulted in high and low maximum likelihood
estimates, respectively. Some of the alternative scenarios, however, were resolved here
and the ancestor of the roquet series had a high maximum likelihood for FPP, even
though parsimony suggested later evolution of FPP.
The ancestor of mainland norops, with the exclusion of the Mexican radiation,
showed 0.936% likelihood for FPP. Therefore, the parsimony scenario of independent
evolution of FPP in the clade of A. nitens is less likely. Within the cristatellus series, the
scenario for evolution in an ancestor of A. cristatellus and sister species was well
supported (ML: 80.1 %). Similarly, a 76.0 % likelihood for the evolution of FPP in the
ancestor of the bimaculatus series supports ancestral evolution of FPP within the series,
but the scenario with independent evolution in A. wattsi was better supported (ML:
90.4%). In the cybotes clade, the best supported scenario was ancestral evolution of FPP
(ML: 84.8%). Finally, the ancestor of the roquet clade, including A. bonairensis, still had
a 69.1% likelihood of FPP. When excluding A. bonairensis, this likelihood rose to 99.5%,
which was consistent with the parsimony result.
21
Geographic analysis
FPP was found throughout the distributional range of anoles (Figure 2). Trinidad
is the only island for which all anole species had FPP. None of the Trinidad anoles are
endemic to this region; all are shared with South America and the other islands of the
Lesser Antilles. Cuba, Hispaniola and most islands of the Lesser Antilles had endemic
species with FPP.
Cuba Of 63 species living on Cuba, there were 11 for which presence of FPP could not
be determined: A. aguera, A. altitudinalis
*
, A. birama, A. delafuenti, A. incredulus
*
, A.
litoralis
*
, A. oporius
*
, A. ruibali
*
, A. terueli
*
, A. toldo
*
, and A. vescus. The majority
belonged to the carolinensis clade (
*
), a radiation for which currently no case of FPP has
been reported. Of 52 remaining species, only three had FPP, two of which were endemic.
Although this was the result of independent evolution of FPP, all female polymorphic
species were members of the sagrei clade. A. birami and A. delafuenti, both members
from this clade, may or may not have polymorphic females. This clade of Cuban anoles is
more closely related to mainland anoles than to the other Cuban anoles. There was no
pattern in the distribution of female polymorphic species across Cuba. A. sagrei is found
throughout Cuba. A. allogus also occurs throughout Cuba, with exception of the islets off
the coast. A. birama is restricted to the central-eastern part of the island.
Hispaniola In Hispaniola, 8 out 43 species were described with FPP, 7 of which were
endemic. A. alumina could be an additional anole with FPP, but uncertainty led us to
include this species as unknown. A. breslini was formerly a subspecies of the female
polymorphic A. whitemani. The presence of FPP in A. breslini will have to be assessed.
The female polymorphic anoles of Hispaniola belonged to four different clades,
22
phylogenetically separated by species from other locations, but all are nested within other
Hispaniola anoles (Figure 1.B.,C.). They all range mostly in the south-western peninsula
(Grand?anse, Sud, Sud-est and Ouest in Ha?ti, and Pedernales in the Dominican
Republic). A. ricordi ranges further into the area along the border between Ha?ti and the
Dominican Republic and into the northwestern peninsula (Nord and Nord-Ouest of
Ha?ti). One species with FPP, A. etheredgi, resides much farther east in the Dominican
Republic. A. cybotes was the only species with FPP that occurs throughout Hispaniola.
Bahamas, Puerto Rico, Jamaica, Cayman Islands The Bahamas and Jamaica do not
have native anoles with FPP. The Cayman Islands hosts the female polymorphic anole A.
conspersus. All these islands share A. sagrei, a species with FPP. Species with FPP
inhabiting Puerto Rico and the Virgin Islands are all shared between both locations.
These are the three closely related anoles A. cristatellus, A. acutus, and A.
ernestwilliamsi, all belonging to the same clade. The two latter species are restricted in
range in Puerto Rico to the bank, while A. cristatellus is more widespread on this island.
In addition, A. cristatellus species has been introduced to Hispaniola.
Lesser Antilles FPP was common in both radiations of the Lesser Antilles, the roquet
clade in the south and bimaculatus series in the north. The northern islands are home to
some species for which the presence of FPP was not known: A. schwartzi, A. pogus and
A. forresti, all former subspecies of the female polymorphic A. wattsi, and A. leachi, a
former subspecies of the non-polymorphic A. bimaculatus.
Remaining islands Two little islands, San Andr?s and Providencia each contain one
species. A. concolor occurs in the former and has FPP. For the species in Providencia, A.
pinchoti, FPP is not known. No species have been described for Aruba. The island of
23
Bonaire is home to A. bonairensis, which does not have FPP. A. townsendi and A.
lineatus, although nested within the mainland norops, are both island species: the former
on Cocos Island off the Costa Rican coast, the latter on Cura?ao. Both are nested within
mainland anoles with FPP, but neither of these island species displayed female
polymorphism.
Mainland Within mainland dactyloa, female polymorphism was rare (Figure 1.D.). For
norops, on the other hand, polymorphism in female dorsal pattern occurred in the
majority of known anoles of the mainland (Figure 1.A.). FPP occurred throughout the
entire geographic distributional range of mainland anoles.
DISCUSSION
We asked the question whether female polymorphism in dorsal patterns (FPP)
was a derived character in anoles and if it could have been the result of parallel evolution.
These questions were approached phylogenetically and geographically.
Phylogenetic patterns in the evolution of FPP in anoles
Both parsimony and maximum likelihood (ML) methods supported the absence of
FPP in the common ancestor of anoles and the consequent occurrence of multiple
evolutionary events along the tree. The current pattern of FPP distribution among anoles
resulted from independent evolution in multiple common ancestors as well as in
individual species. The shared state of FPP by closely related species resulting from
ancestral evolutionary events explains why a phylogenetic signal was found.
Nevertheless, FPP arose in several ancestors independently, so that phylogenetic
24
relationships alone could not explain the complete distribution pattern of FPP, as
indicated by the moderate value of lambda.
Similar to forward evolution of FPP, some losses of FPP occurred in ancestors,
while others happened in single species. In spite of the much slower rate of loss
compared to gains, independent losses were seen in both mainland clades, and depending
on the evolutionary scenario, losses were seen in other clades as well. These scenarios
were well supported by the ML method. Once lost, FPP was not regained, except in A.
woodi. The possibility to re-gain a trait after it was lost remains controversial
(Cunningham 1999; Dollo 1893; Simpson 1953). Following the phylogeny of Poe (2004),
the scenario where FPP evolves independently in A. woodi was not supported. Regardless
of the opinion on reversals, or some differences between phylogenies, our major
conclusion remains that multiple independent evolutionary events explain the current
distribution of FPP among anoles. Based on our dataset, FPP has evolved at least fifteen
times independently in anoles, and it has been lost at least five times. To reconstruct our
full scenario of the evolutionary history of FPP, either the number of losses or the number
of gains increases even further. In addition, the number of losses and gains are expected
to increase as more species can be added to the analysis. The multiple independent
evolutionary events suggest that FPP in anoles may have evolved in response to a
common environment (Harvey and Pagel 1991; Larson and Losos 1996; Schluter 2000),
a hypothesis that needs to be tested.
Any analysis based on evolutionary relationships among species relies on the
accuracy of the available phylogeny. Future changes in phylogenetic inference could
therefore affect the results presented here. Nevertheless, we expect the main conclusions
25
concerning the evolution of female polymorphism in anoles to be robust for several
reasons. First, differences between recent phylogenies did not drastically affect the
evolutionary path of female polymorphism, especially in the case of maximum likelihood
estimates. Next, future changes to the hypothesis of evolutionary relationships in Anolis
lizards are expected to be minor (Nicholson et al. 2005). Last, a large proportion of the
anoles of the West Indies were included, and thus the number of independent
evolutionary events is unlikely to change greatly. Presence of FPP could have been
underestimated because it was only based on species accounts. Consideration of the
thorough studies on Caribbean anoles with highly detailed descriptions of variations in
dorsal patterns by multiple authors suggests that there will be few changes, so that the
number of independent events would hold. For the mainland, conclusions are less certain,
because fewer anoles are known. Nevertheless, mainland species are all members of the
clades norops or dactyloa, so that new species found on the mainland are likely to belong
to either of these clades (Nicholson et al. 2005). The high proportion of female
polymorphism among known species or the norops clade suggests that more polymorphic
species are likely to be found within this radiation and a polymorphic common ancestor
for mainland norops is thus very probable. Any further occurrence of female polymorphic
dactyloa, on the other hand, are likely the result of independent evolutionary events.
Location and timing of the evolution of female polymorphism
Our results show that FPP evolved in several ancestors and divergence estimates
for those ancestors may provide an estimate for how long FPP has been present in those
clades. Within dactyloa from the southern Lesser Antilles, for example, the deepest split
was estimated between 15.5 and 17 MYA (Creer et al. 2001). The bimaculatus series
26
from the northern Lesser Antilles was estimated to have diverged between 7.9 and 9.7
MYA (Thorpe et al. 2008). Within this clade, the female polymorphic sister species A.
marmoratus and A. sabanus were estimated to have diverged from their common
ancestor ca. 1.8-3.6 MYA (Stenson et al. 2004). These estimates are based on molecular
clocks and should be approached with caution (e.g. Bromham 2002; Crother and Guyer
1996; Graur and Martin 2004). Nonetheless, the estimates indicate that FPP has been
present in the anole radiation for a few million years. Continued presence of
polymorphism over such a long period of time is an indicator for the presence of
stabilizing selection, as random processes or directional selection are expected to result in
fixation (Futuyma 1998).
Geographic isolation and the evolution of female polymorphism
The geographic distribution of anoles should be particularly favorable for the
occurrence of parallel evolution. Anoles inhabit nearly all islands in the West Indies,
most of the Neotropical mainland and a few islands in the Pacific Ocean. As the
phylogeny shows, anoles have mostly speciated within islands and within the mainland,
so that closely related species often inhabit the same region. This pattern of geographic
isolation is thought to increase the number of times a trait can evolve independently, if
similar environments are encountered in the separate locations (Schluter 2000; Simpson
1953; Wiens et al. 2009). Moreover, anoles are known for their rapid evolution in
response to their environment (Losos et al. 2006b; Malhotra and Thorpe 1991). Indeed,
geographic distribution patterns of anoles have been used to explain repeated evolution of
some of their morphological characteristics in response to habitat use and competition
(Harmon et al. 2005; Losos 1992b; Losos et al. 2006a; Losos et al. 1998; Rand and
27
Williams 1969; Williams 1972). The phylogenetic and geographic distribution pattern of
FPP indicates that geographic isolation may have also contributed to the number of times
FPP arose in anoles; the trait evolved separately in the majority of the islands and on the
mainland.
Current geographic boundaries, however, were not necessarily present during the
early evolution of anoles and the evolution of FPP. Cuba, Hispaniola and Puerto Rico
used to be part of a larger landmass called the proto-Antilles (Pindell 1985; Rosen 1975).
Some debate revolves around the processes behind the current distribution pattern of
anole species, due to apparent discrepancies between the timing of anole evolution and
geological history (Reviewed by Pregill and Crother 1999). Overwater dispersal events
form the central issue in the debate, especially for speciation on the Greater Antilles,
which could have resulted from vicariance when the Proto-Antilles split or from
overwater dispersal after the split (Burnell and Hedges 1990; Crother and Guyer 1996;
Guyer and Crother 1996; Hedges 1996; Hedges 2006; Hedges et al. 1994; See also
Pregill and Crother 1999). Regardless, at least a few overwater dispersals must have
occurred. Jamaica, for example, was submerged until the late Oligocene or early Miocene
(Buskirk 1985; Iturralde-Vinent 2006; Robinson 1994). Current anoles on Jamaica form a
monophyletic clade and are thus likely the result of one recent overwater dispersal event
(Crother and Guyer 1996; Hedges and Burnell 1990). The Cayman Islands, the Bahamas
and St. Croix are oceanic islands that were never connected to the mainland and could
thus only have been reached via water.
Overwater dispersal could help explain the distinct events of FPP evolution on
many islands, and why it never arose on Jamaica, the Bahamas and St. Croix. To colonize
28
islands, survival of a few individuals from dispersal events would suffice. Female
polymorphism, on the other hand, would require multiple alleles and this variation must
be represented in the founder population for polymorphism to be maintained.
Consequently, a larger number of individuals would have had to reach the new location.
Although such scenario is not impossible, long distance overwater dispersal would likely
lead to a small founder population and consequent loss of alleles (Gorman and Kim 1976;
Gorman et al. 1978). This idea is supported by the absence of FPP on Cocos Island and
Cura?ao, two islands that were populated by descendants from mainland norops in which
FPP was found to be common (Williams 1969). The abundance of FPP among mainland
norops fits this theory, because mainland norops are thought to be the result of a single
island-to-mainland overwater dispersal event (Jackman et al. 1997; Nicholson 2002;
Nicholson et al. 2005). FPP likely evolved shortly after the ancestor had reached the
mainland and was only lost in a few species, including those migrating to islands.
On the Lesser Antilles too, FPP was found to be maintained after it arose in an
ancestor, although this was the result of two independent events. The female polymorphic
island dactyloa only occupy the southern islands up to Martinique. The islands from
Dominica north to Anguilla are inhabited by the polymorphic bimaculatus series, which
are more closely related to Hispaniolan and Puerto Rican species, and reached the Lesser
Antilles from the north (Gorman and Atkins 1969; Guyer and Savage 1986; Williams
1969). In both clades, FPP evolved in a common ancestor. Overwater dispersal between
islands must have occurred here too, so why was FPP retained here? We hypothesize that
a shorter migration distance could have increased the chance for FPP to be maintained. A
shorter distance might increase the chance for adults (rather than eggs) to migrate and
29
some variation is thought to survive bottlenecks in anoles because females can store
sperm of multiple males (Eales et al. 2008). Shorter migration distance would also
facilitate multiple migration events, which can contribute to maintaining variation (Kolbe
et al. 2007; Kolbe et al. 2004; Kolbe et al. 2008). To test these ideas, however, historical
locations of the islands along with timing of speciation events are required.
The evolutionary history of FPP on Cuba, Hispaniola and Puerto Rico shows that
FPP was absent in the common ancestor of the clades that established on these islands.
Generally, FPP here evolved in species that diverged relatively recently compared to the
first establishment of each of the clades. This could have resulted from loss of allelic
variance in the founder population. Our analysis, however, suggests that FPP may not
have been present in the source population. Either scenario is consistent with Simpson?s
(1953) hypothesis that presence of similar environments on geographically isolated
locations would stimulate parallel evolution. Under this hypothesis, FPP would have
evolved as speciation occurred and species occupied environments that were conducive
of maintaining variation in dorsal patterns of females.
Geographic isolation may thus have contributed to the number of independent
events in the evolution of FPP in anoles. The major question, however, becomes why it
evolved (and disappeared) repeatedly. The repeated evolution of a similar trait is not
unlikely with (relatively) closely related species, given their similar genetic composition
and ecological environment (Haldane 1932). To distinguish between random processes
and selective pressures resulting in parallel evolution, FPP will have to be approached
through comparative studies to identify a possible common environment, and further
30
investigation at the population level will be required to understand how FPP is
maintained.
Another interesting pattern that emerged when combining phylogenetic and
geographic analyses is the evolution of FPP in norops and dactyloa. Both clades contain
island as well as mainland species. In norops, FPP is abundant among mainland species,
but rare in island species. Nevertheless, the Cuban species of the norops clade are the
only species known to have FPP on Cuba, even though more than 60 species, divided
over several separate radiations inhabit Cuba. The pattern was opposite in dactyloa. Only
one mainland dactyloan anole in our dataset was female polymorphic, while among the
Lesser Antilles relatives (roquet) absence of FPP was rare. Interestingly, the distantly
related anole radiation that occupies the Northern Lesser Antilles (bimaculatus) contains
many female polymorphic species as well. The relatively sparse occurrence of female
polymorphism in Cuba and abundant occurrence in the Lesser Antilles in two distantly
related clades is consistent with the idea of a possibly shared environment. The absence
of female polymorphism in dactyloa of the mainland, where the only other radiation is
almost entirely polymorphic, poses a dilemma that deservers further investigation.
Summary
Our analyses presented a framework of opportunities and limitations for further
study of female polymorphism in anoles. Female polymorphism of dorsal patterns in
anoles was found to be partially associated with phylogenetic relationships between
species, and further analyses should account for this dependence on evolutionary history.
Phylogenetic relationships, however, did not fully explain the current distribution of
female polymorphism among anoles; multiple independent evolutionary events occurred
31
in nearly every geographically isolated location within the distributional range of anoles.
In the Greater Antilles, evolution of FPP happened relatively recently within each clade
that contains female polymorphic species, but on the Lesser Antilles and the mainland,
FPP evolved in an early ancestor of modern species. Repeated independent evolution
along with the repeated evolution after founding radiations in geographically isolated
locations are indicative of an adaptive nature for FPP. If this pattern is indeed the result
of parallel evolution in response to selective pressures, than female polymorphic anoles
are expected to share similar environments. Moreover, female polymorphism should
disappear in species that evolve in different habitats from their polymorphic relatives, a
hypothesis that remains to be tested.
32
Table 1. Nomenclature applied for clades within Anolis. Node numbers of the phylogeny
of Poe (2004) are included to indicate the specific clade on the tree.
Clade name
Node number
(Poe 2004) Series
Norops
= Beta anoles
(Etheridge 1960).
286
Cuba: sagrei series (node: 225 )
Jamaica: grahami series (node: 284)
Cybotes 293
Ctenonotus 216
Northern Lesser Antilles: bimaculatus series
Greater Antilles: cristatellus series.
Carolinensis 309
Chamaelinorops 197
Xiphosurus 190
Equestris 324
Dactyloa 352
Southern Lesser Antilles: roquet series (node:
351)
33
Figure 1. Evolution of female polymorphism (FPP) in anoles. A. norops clade, B.
cybotus and ctenonotus clades, C. carolinensis, chamaelinorops, xiphosurus and equestris
clades, D. dactyloa clade. Presence of FPP is indicated with grey dots. Pie diagrams are
shown for ancestors, with the proportion of black representing the likelihood of FPP
being present in this ancestor. Species distributions are given after their name: Jam:
Jamaica, GA: Greater Antilles, LA: Lesser Antilles (minus VI), MEX: Mexico, PR:
Puerto Rico, NCAM: North Central America, CAM: Central America, SAM: South
America, VI: Virgin Islands. The possible number of gains and losses in parsimony
analysis is shown as ?gains:losses?.
A.
34
B.
35
C.
36
37
D.
Figure 2. Geographic distribution of female polymorphism in anoles. The total number of species per location are given along with the
proportion of polymorphic (endemic: black, non-endemic: dark-grey), proportion of species for which the presence of polymorphism
was unknown (light yellow) and proportion of non-polymorphic species (white). For the two radiations of mainland anoles, the
proportion of polymorphic species is given relative to the number of species for which the presence of polymorphism was known.
38
CHAPTER THREE
DORSAL PATTERN POLYMORPHISM AND HABITAT USE IN FEMALE
ANOLES (Squamata: Polychrotidae)
INTRODUCTION
Color pattern polymorphism has long been the classic model for tests of natural
and sexual selection (Darwin 1859; Darwin 1871). Because we expect selective pressures
to favor a particular morph, selection should lead to one optimal male and female morph
(Charlesworth 1984; Charlesworth 1987; Fisher 1930). Even without selective pressures,
however, variation is expected to disappear through chance events. Thus, intra-sexual
polymorphism challenges evolutionary theory, and understanding mechanisms for the
evolution and maintenance of alternative morphs provides insight into the processes of
natural and sexual selection and drift. Because color patterns can be readily observed,
they provide ideal study subjects to address these challenges.
Much research has been devoted to color pattern polymorphism (e.g. Basolo
2006; Galeotti et al. 2003; Gray and McKinnon 2007; Hoffman and Blouin 2000; Houde
1997; Jones et al. 1977; Roulin 2004; Seehausen et al. 1999). Gender-based
polymorphism, however, has mostly focused on males. Many occurrences of male color
polymorphism are explained by mate choice and mating strategy (Shuster and Wade
2003). A popular example is the rock-paper-scissor game of male color morphs in the
39
lizard Uta stansburiana (Sinervo and Lively 1996). Female color polymorphism
was studied intensively in the early 20
th
century, with regards to M?llerian mimics in
butterflies where males were monomorphic, but females displayed a variety of patterns to
mimic different non-palatable species (Fisher and Ford 1929; Owen 1971; Wickler 1968).
It was not until the last few decades, however, that female polymorphism re-gained
interest, much due to work on damselflies (Svensson et al. 2009). Female damselflies
vary in the color of their abdomen, and often one color pattern resembles the male?s
(Abbott and Svensson 2005; Tillyard 1917). Several hypotheses have been proposed and
tested, such as male harassment, frequency or density dependent mating success and
balancing predation (Andres et al. 2002; Cordero 1992; Cordero et al. 1998; Hinnekint
and Dumont 1989; Miller and Fincke 1999; Robertson 1985; Sherratt 2001). In spite of
extensive research, much remains to be investigated. Interestingly, a recent study found
that female polymorphism is the ancestral state for at least two genera of damselflies
(Van Gossum and Mattern 2008). This, of course, changes the conclusions that can be
drawn from population based studies. Even though female polymorphism in damselflies
appears to be adaptive (Abbott et al. 2008; Andres et al. 2000), only current mechanisms
for maintenance can be addressed. Moreover, studying the loss of this trait should be
more informative when the trait is primitive.
The only other organisms for which the evolutionary history of female
polymorphism has been investigated are anoles (Chapter 2). Female polymorphism in
anoles consists of multiple dorsal patterns within a population (Savage 2002). Unlike
many other cases of female color pattern polymorphism, the females do not differ in
color. Moreover, the patterns appear cryptic, without colors that contrast with natural
40
backgrounds and patterns that are formed by the same colors as seen on the rest of the
body. Contrary to findings for damselflies, female polymorphism in anoles was shown to
result from multiple independent events, although some closely related species share a
female polymorphic ancestor (Chapter 2). This suggests that female polymorphism might
be the result of parallel evolution, a hypothesis that needs to be tested. Parallel evolution
occurs when distantly related species share a common environment that allows for a
shared trait, in this case female polymorphism, to evolve. This is similar to convergent
evolution, except that the new trait evolves from a similar ancestral state in parallel
evolution (Zhang and Kumar 1997).
In explaining polymorphism of cryptic color patterns, habit use is an important
central issue, because visibility of color patterns is determined by the background against
which they are seen (Endler 1978; Merilaita and Jormalainen 1997). Visibility of an
individual is determined by its color pattern, its behavior and the background; and also by
the visual scanning behavior of the observer (Endler 1978). Light environment plays an
important role in the visibility of color patterns, as well as time of day, cloud cover and
vegetation cover; all affect the light environment (Endler 1993). Within a particular
habitat, light environment can differ spatially with type of vegetation, and seasonally. An
individual can ?control? the light environment behaviorally through micro-habitat choice.
Additionally, color patterns can serve three functions in animals: thermoregulation, inter
and intra-specific communication, and evasion of predators (Cott 1940; Endler 1978).
Thermoregulation, communication and predator avoidance can interact, and the resulting
color pattern is not necessarily optimized for only one of these factors (Endler 1978;
Endler 1980; Merilaita et al. 2001). Because color patterns are visual signals, habitat use
41
can play an important role regardless of the functions of the color patterns. Thus, if
female polymorphism is adaptive, we would expect to see a relationship between the
presence of this trait and habitat use.
We investigated the relationship between female polymorphism and habitat use in
anoles, using comparative methods. The need for comparative approaches to study
maintenance of color pattern polymorphism has been emphasized by Gray and
McKinnon (2007), but only few studies have done so (Galeotti et al. 2003; Seehausen et
al. 1999; Van Gossum and Mattern 2008). Due to the complexity of habitats, we
incorporated micro-and macro-habitat variables, and expected to find an association
between the presence of female polymorphism and at least one habitat aspect.
METHODS
Anoline lizards (Squamata: Polychrotidae) form a radiation of nearly 400 species.
Due to the standing debate on nomenclature of anoles and the need for a practical
approach, we applied the original genus name Anolis for all species, but referred to
specific clades as detailed elsewhere (Chapter 2). Anoles range as far north as the
southeastern United States and are found on the Carribean islands and the Neotropical
mainland. Many species, on the islands as well as on the mainland, are female
polymorphic (Chapter 2). Females of these species vary in distinctive dorsal patterns and
typical female patterns in those species include a light vertebral stripe, diamonds or dark
chevrons. Although some variation exists, the same general patterns occur across species
(Savage 2002).
Data on dorsal patterns, body size, distribution and habitat use were compiled
from books, review papers and the primary literature (Avila-Pires 1995; Campbell 1998 ;
42
Cast et al. 2000; Dixon and Soini 1986; Duellman 1978; Duellman 2005; Estrada and
Hedges 1995; Fitch 1973; Fitch 1975; Garrido and Hedges 2001; Irschick et al. 1997;
Knox et al. 2001; Lazell 1972; Leal and Losos 2000; Lee 1996; Lee 2000; Lenart et al.
1997; Losos and DeQueiroz 1997; Moermond 1979; Nicholson et al. 2001; Pinto et al.
2008; Rivero 1998; Savage 2002; Schwartz and Henderson 1991; Stafford and Meyer
2000; Stejneger 1900; Thomas et al. 2009; van Buurt 2005; Vitt and de la Torre 1996).
Geographical variation, subspecies variation or variation due to metachromatism (color
change) was not considered polymorphic. Because descriptions were at species level,
polymorphism could not be assessed for separate populations.
Similarly, habitat use was assessed for the species as a whole. To represent the
overall light environment individuals could be exposed to, we combined micro-and
macro-habitat data with body size. The macro-habitat data reflected the overall light
environment available to the species. Macro-habitats were based on habitat use
descriptions or point occurrence data and vegetation maps. Habitats were categorized as:
?old growth forest?, ?secondary forest?, ?open natural habitat?, ?agriculture? and ?urban?.
Open natural habitat included shrub, beaches, savanna and grassland. Agriculture
included mostly plantations, e.g. coffee or cacao. Urban species dwelled in gardens,
houses or other man-made structures. This habitat division was ranked in this order as a
combined variable to represent openness and disturbance. Because one species can use
more than one type of habitat, habitat use was summarized into two variables based on
habitat: number of different habitats used and maximum level of disturbance. Clearly,
these were collinear and only one could be used in our statistical model. Another
43
variable, humidity, was also based on habitat use descriptions and consisted of ?dry?, ?dry
to moist? and ?moist to wet?.
Using distribution maps, species were assigned to a rainfall pattern category. This
variable was based on the Koppen-Geiger classification data and indicated the amount
and timing of rainfall (Kottek et al. 2006). Rainfall pattern was included because some
habitats change drastically throughout the lifetime of an anole based on rain patterns. The
distribution of anoles in our dataset contained four major Koppen-Geiger classes: tropical
rainforest climate with no natural seasons (Af), tropical monsoon climate with slightly
more variation in precipitation (Am), tropical wet and dry or savanna climate with a clear
wet and dry season (Aw), and a dry climate with even less rainfall (BSh). These were
divided into four categories. The first category included non-seasonal environments only
(Af). The second category included only wet-dry environment (Aw). Species were
assigned to the third category when they occupied both non-seasonal and seasonal
environments (Af+Aw). Species occupying areas with a dry climate were assigned to the
fourth category (BSh). All species ranging into habitat with slight variation in rainfall
(Am) also occupied either non-seasonal or seasonal environments and were assigned to
one of those categories. In summary, macro-habitat use was divided into: maximum
disturbance level, number of different habitat types occupied, rainfall pattern, and
humidity.
Within a macro-habitat, a multitude of microhabitats are available. Microhabitat
use reflects the specific light environment and background within the macro-habitat to
which the species is exposed. In addition, body size indicates how much of the variation
within a patch to which an individual can be exposed at one time. For body size, the
44
maximum value for females was recorded. For microhabitat use of Greater Antilles
species, ecomorph type (Williams 1972; Williams 1983) could be used. This represents
habitat use as well as morphological and behavioral characteristics. For other species the
relationships between these characteristics is often different (Irschick et al. 1997; Knox et
al. 2001), so that many species have not been assigned to an ecomorph type. Therefore,
we used perch use descriptions instead. The first category included species occupying
tree canopies (ecomorphs: crown, trunk-crown). The second category consisted of trunk
perchers (ecomorphs: trunk, trunk-ground, trunk-bush). Twig perchers were placed in
their own category, because although they use thin perches, they could be perching low in
bushes as well as high in the canopy. Species perching on bushy vegetation formed a
fourth category (bush, grass-bush). Atypical perch users, such as aquatic species and rock
dwellers were included in a fifth category. After collecting the data, it appeared that none
of the anoles in the last category had female polymorphism and therefore this category
was excluded from the analyses of polymorphism.
Statistical Analysis
Female polymorphism in anoles was shared by some closely related species.
Therefore, the data were not independent and a phylogenetic correlation structure had to
be incorporated. We used a combined phylogeny from the most recent and extensive
works (Nicholson et al. 2005; Poe 2004). Equal branch lengths were assumed.
Generalized estimating equations (GEE) with a binomial distribution and the phylogeny
as a correlation matrix were applied to a model with the occurrence of female
polymorphism as the response variable (Table 1). Model selection started with a
complete model and proceeded by sequentially removing the term with the highest non-
45
significant P-value. This process was repeated until all terms were significant. For
comparison, a logistic regression with the same terms was run as well. This model did not
incorporate the phylogeny, but was used to indicate whether phylogenetic relationships
affected the occurrence of FPP across habitats. The model selection process was identical
to the process in GEE, except that an AIC was used for dropping terms rather than the P-
value. Because maximum disturbance and number of habitats were collinear, the model
was run with each term separately. Maximum disturbance consistently resulted in a
model with a higher Akaike?s Information Criterion (AIC) than the model with number
of habitats in the logistic regression. In GEE, maximum disturbance turned out
insignificant as well. Thus, only the analysis with number of habitats is shown. Because
many occurrences of FPP result from ancestral evolution in the mainland norops clade
(Chapter 2), we repeated the analysis with a reduced number of species from the norops
clade. In particular, we collapsed the node leading to A. sericeus and remaining norops
species. This replaced 15 species of the norops clade by one species with the
characteristics of A. sericeus.
A post-hoc Levene?s test was incorporated to investigate whether variance in SVL
between polymorphic and non-polymorphic species was significantly different. All
analyses were performed in R (R Development Core Team 2008). The GEE was
completed with the packages ?ape? and ?gee? (Carey 2007; Paradis et al. 2004). Levene?s
test was performed in the ?car? package (Fox 2009). Relevant habitat data were plotted
onto a phylogeny using the ?geiger? package (Harmon et al. 2008).
46
RESULTS
Female dorsal pattern polymorphism (FPP) evolved in several clades of Anolis
(Figure 1). In the norops clade, the lack of FPP was uncommon. Among 121 species
included in the dataset, 37 scored positive for female polymorphism. The most common
form of polymorphism, nearly 40% of the cases, was the occurrence of females with a
vertebral stripe alongside females with the male pattern. About 25% of female
polymorphic species had a male pattern-stripe-diamond combination of female patterns,
but this was only found in mainland species, while a stripe-diamond combination without
male pattern (16%) was only found in island species. Male patterns of female
polymorphic species were often indistinct, without pattern or with dorsal blotches.
After removal of the non-polymorphic species in the category of atypical perches,
110 species were used to analyze FPP. The logistic regression model dropped humidity
and snout-vent length, respectively, since they had little influence on FPP (Table 1). This
led to a model with perch type, number of habitats and rainfall pattern. The number of
habitats was included, because the model without this variable was not significantly
better than the model with this variable. The GEE resulted in a similar model, but rainfall
pattern became insignificant. Humidity, snout-vent length and rainfall pattern were
dropped from the model consecutively (Table 1). The remaining model consisted of
perch type and number of habitats used. Collapsing the node leading to A. sericeus and
related mainland species of the norops clade led to similar results (not shown).
Plotting the data illustrated the results from the models (Figure 2). Female
polymorphism occurred across different perch types, but was most common in the trunk
perching species and was very rare in twig perching anoles. Among mainland species of
47
the norops clade, FPP was not present in species that were not perching on trunks or on
bushy vegetation (Figure 1). This pattern did not continue throughout the rest of the
phylogeny and also anoles using other perch types were polymorphic (Figure 1). Among
twig anoles, only one species was polymorphic in this study: A. oculatus. It is closely
related to the polymorphic species A. marmoratus and A. sabanus, which perch low on
trunks and bushes. Polymorphic canopy species were mostly members of the roquet
series in the dactyloa clade (Figure 1.C). The mainland dactyloa in our analysis were also
canopy species, but were not polymorphic.
Polymorphic species were most common in geographic areas without true seasons
and in areas with a dry and wet season (?Mixed? in Figure 2). For the number of habitats,
differences in the occurrence of female polymorphism were not as clear, but the lowest
proportion of polymorphic species was found among species restricted to one or two
habitats, while the highest proportion was among species using three or five different
habitats. The latter category, however, was a small sample of species. Species occurring
in three different habitats were mostly species found in both forest and natural, open
areas. Female polymorphism was rare among species occupying only dry habitats,
although proportions of female polymorphism were not significantly different across
categories of humidity. Snout-vent length (SVL) did not differ between species with and
without female polymorphism, but species with polymorphism showed less variation in
snout-vent length. (S.D
.(No FPP)
= 39.7, S.D.
(FPP)
= 20.2, F
(1,111)
= 4.80, P = 0.0306).
DISCUSSION
We found a strong association between the occurrence of FPP and some aspects
of habitat use, even though FPP showed significant phylogenetic signal (Chapter 2). In
48
particular, our strongest result supported by all analyses indicated that anoles perching on
trunks were more likely to be female polymorphic. Furthermore, FPP was more common
among species occupying three different habitat types and occurring in both
environments with and without seasonality in rainfall. The latter was phylogenetically
associated, and patterns with rainfall disappeared when phylogenetic relationships were
accounted for. Rainfall patterns were based on K?ppen-Geiger classifications, which
encompass large regions, without accounting for smaller regional variation. Because
closely related species often occur on the same island or in the same area, it is not
surprising that the effect of rainfall disappeared when effects of phylogeny were
removed. To discern whether the relationship between rainfall pattern and polymorphism
is solely due to phylogeny, rainfall patterns and polymorphism could be studied at a
smaller scale. For the purpose of the current analysis, however, effects remaining after
incorporating phylogeny provide an indication for a similar environment among female
polymorphic species, regardless of their affiliation. Such association is expected if FPP is
adaptive.
There was a trend of increasing occurrence of polymorphism with increased
number of habitats occupied. Proportion of polymorphism was highest for species
occupying three habitats and most of these species used forests as well as open habitats.
This hints at a connection between variety of habitats occupied and presence of FPP,
supporting the hypothesis that species occupying spatially or temporally heterogeneous
habitats are more likely to be polymorphic (Cain and Sheppard 1954b; Ford 1945;
Hedrick et al. 1976). When incorporating human-altered habitat (i.e. 4 or 5 habitats
occupied), however, there was no consistent trend. Likely, FPP evolved before habitats
49
had been altered. In addition, human altered habitats may consist of elements similar to
natural habitats when considering light environments. The variation in light environment
may thus be more important than the level of disturbance, but assessment of similarities
between human-altered and natural habitat would require more detailed measurements of
habitat use.
Perch use appeared to influence the occurrence of FPP, regardless of phylogenetic
relationships. None of the species on rocks, in aquatic environments or other atypical
perches were female polymorphic. For analytical purposes, these species had to be
removed for analysis, and thus the statistical results may have underestimated the
relationship between FPP and perch use. The majority of FPP was seen in anoles
perching on trunks. Interestingly, in the mainland radiation of the norops clade, FPP only
occurred in anoles perching on trunks or bushy vegetation, and was lost in all others.
These anoles share a common ancestor that was likely already female polymorphic
(Chapter 2). The loss of FPP in this radiation thus provides a stronger indication of any
association with habitat variables than the mere presence of FPP. Among the remainder
of anoles, however, the relationship between perch use and FPP was not as strict,
although the association of FPP with habitat (including perch type) remained after
removing the FPP-rich radiation of the mainland norops clade. Among island anoles, not
all trunk perching anoles were polymorphic, and FPP was also found for other perch
types, mostly in the dactyloa radiation. Species from both the mainland and island
dactyloa radiations in our analysis were canopy species, but only among the island
radiation was FPP found to be common. The mainland lineage of dactyloa has at least
one polymorphic member, A. casildae, not included in the analysis for lack of detailed
50
habitat data. Unlike the other species, however, this species appears to be a low perching
anole (Nicholson et al. 2001).
For some species, perch use varies between or within populations and
classification may be somewhat misleading. Here, we highlight a few examples that are
relevant to our analysis. Some Lesser Antilles species that are considered canopy species,
are also found on lower perches. For example, A. marmoratus and A. sabanus
morphologically resemble canopy species (Losos and DeQueiroz 1997), but perch
behavior is more similar to trunk-ground species, although some variation exists between
populations of A. marmoratus (Schwartz and Henderson 1991). A. oculatus is a diverse
species and some populations seem to be closer to trunk-crown anoles (our canopy
category), while others classify closer to trunk-ground species (our trunk category) (Knox
et al. 2001). Here we classified it as ?twig? and it would be interesting to check whether
female polymorphism differs between populations in this species. Females of A.
conspersus behave more like trunk-ground anoles with regards to perch choice (Schoener
1967), even though this species is generally considered a crown anole. A. aeneus,
classified as a trunk-crown anole (?canopy?) (Losos and DeQueiroz 1997), is also found
in scrub habitat, which may be closer to our ?bush? category.
The pattern of evolution of FPP in association with habitat, in particular perch
type, suggests that FPP requires a particular environment for FPP to evolve and be
maintained. The main question becomes why it evolved mostly in trunk perching species.
Predation has been suggested to explain the multiple dorsal patterns in female anoles
(Macedonia 2001; Schoener and Schoener 1976). Anoles are preyed upon by mammals,
birds, snakes, lizards including congeners, spiders and other invertebrates (e.g., Guyer
51
1988a; Henderson and Crother 1989; McLaughlin and Roughgarden 1989; Reagan 1996).
Perhaps anoles perching on trunks are more exposed to predators. A predation hypothesis
is further supported by the presence of a vertebral stripe as one of the patterns in female
polymorphic anoles. In fact, the vertebral stripe was the most common alternative pattern
in female polymorphic anoles, while males of these species generally had indistinct
patterns or vertebral blotches. The vertebral stripe is a common alternative dorsal pattern
in polymorphic reptiles and often related to habitat use as a concealing or disruptive
pattern (Cott 1940; Duellman and Trueb 1986; Hoffman and Blouin 2000; Patterson and
Daugherty 1990; Schoener and Schoener 1976).
Based on a few examples, Schoener and Schoener (1976) assumed that female
polymorphic species were small, and suggested that the vertebral stripe would only be
effective as a predation avoidance mechanism in smaller species. We found no
association, however, between SVL and the presence of FPP. Interestingly, SVL
variation was much smaller in polymorphic species, suggesting that female patterns are
only concealing within a range of sizes from about 40-100 mm. Females of the largest
species with FPP, A. ricordi, grow up to 151 mm SVL. The dorsum of these females is
either rather uniform or shows a dark reticulate pattern, but not the vertebral stripe as
seen in most other polymorphic species. The next largest polymorphic species was N.
capito, with females measuring up to 100 mm SVL. Here, a vertebral stripe is one of the
alternative patterns. Thus, although FPP is not limited to small species, size appears to
matter and perhaps Schoener and Schoener?s (1976) hypothesis could be adjusted to
apply to a vertebral stripe as being concealing only in anoles of certain sizes. Whether the
stripe indeed reduces predation would have to be tested.
52
The other question arising from our results is why the association between perch
use and FPP in island radiations was not as strict compared to the mainland anoles. This
dichotomy between island and mainland anoles also exists in the origination of FPP, with
mostly independent evolution of FPP in island clades and ancestral evolution of FPP for
mainland norops species. It was suggested that the genetic variation associated with FPP
disappears as a consequence of founder effects upon colonization of new territories
(Chapter 2). Mainland norops species are the result of one colonization event event
(Jackman et al. 1997; Nicholson 2002; Nicholson et al. 2005). For island anoles, the
variation necessary for FPP to evolve may not have been present in all species that
occupy habitats where we would expect FPP to occur.
Selective pressures may also differ between the Neotropical mainland and the
islands of the West Indies. The majority of the West Indies vertebrate fauna is composed
of anoles, unlike the mainland where these lizards constitute only a minor part of the
overall faunal biomass (Rand and Humphrey 1968), and biological interactions such as
competition or predation are expected to differ (e.g. Andrews 1976; Andrews 1979;
Wright 1981). Anoles on the mainland may experience higher predation than anoles on
the islands (Andrews 1979; Andrews 1991; Schoener 1985; Schoener and Schoener
1982), because there is probably a wider variety of species preying on anoles (Losos
2009). Nevertheless, some island species may still experience high predation pressure.
For instance, snakes of the genus Alsophis (Squamata: Colubridae) are anole specialists,
and can be very abundant in the West Indies (Henderson and Sajdak 1996). In the Lesser
Antilles most islands are inhabited by only one or two species, and predators are thus
limited in their choice as to which species to prey on. Species on the Lesser Antilles
53
could therefore experience high predation rates. Indeed, predation by pearly-eyed trashers
on the female polymorphic A. wattsi was found to be high and this was related to moist
habitats (McLaughlin and Roughgarden 1989).
In summary, our results suggest that female polymorphic species share similar
environments, but the broad variables incorporated in our analysis did not allow us to
determine which elements within this environment are important for FPP. When
combining our results with circumstantial evidence, we can hypothesize that predation is
likely to be important in the maintenance of FPP.
54
Table 1. Model selection steps to determine the relationship between macro and micro-
habitat variables and the presence of female polymorphism. Both the logistic regression
model and the model based on generalized estimating equations are shown. The term
with the highest P-value was dropped. The model was then run again without this term to
select the next highest non-significant term. The process was repeated until the final
model only contained significant terms. Drop-terms are listed in order of being dropped.
AIC values represent values in comparison with the final model with only the respective
term being dropped. Model selection with generalized estimating equations was based on
P-values.
Multiple regression df AIC P
Full model: FPP~svl+Perch+Humidity+Habitats+Rainfall 137.919
Least significant term dropped:
Humidity 2 0.685
Snout-vent length (svl) 1 0.474
Final model: FPP~Perch+Habitats+Rainfall 133.187
Rainfall pattern (?Rainfall?) 3 142.046 0.002
Perch 142.599 0.002
Number of different habitats 4 133.279 0.088
Generalized Estimating Equations - Incorporating phylogeny df P
Full model: FPP~svl+Perch+Humidity+Habitats+Rainfall
Humidity 2 0.532
Snout-vent length (svl) 1 0.239
Rainfall pattern (?Rainfall?) 3 0.272
Final model: FPP~Perch+Habitats
Number of different habitats (?Habitats?) 4 0.002
Perch 3 < 0.0001
55
Figure 1. Phylogeny with patterns and habitat variables related to distribution of female
polymorphism in anoles. Female polymorphic species are indicated with a star (*). The
second column indicates perch use: bushy (bu), twig (tw), trunk (tr), canopy (ca) and
other (oo). Rainfall patterns are in the third column: no distinct season (N), wet and dry
season (Y), N+Y (Mix) and dry regions (Dry). The last column presents the number of
habitats used. A. norops clade, B. cybotes, ctenonotus, carolinensis, chamaelinorops,
xiphosurus and equestris clades, C. dacyloa clade.
A
56
B
57
C
58
Figure 2. Distribution of female polymorphism among habitat types. The variables Perch
Use, Rainfall Pattern and Number of Habitats were found to be significant determinants
in the occurrence of female polymorphism (grey). The width of each column reflects the
proportion of species (total =121 species) in each category. For details: see text.
59
CHAPTER FOUR
SURVIVAL OF ALTERNATIVE DORSAL PATTERN MORPHS
IN FEMALES OF THE ANOLE NOROPS HUMILIS.
INTRODUCTION
The occurrence of multiple color patterns within a population has long fascinated
evolutionary biologists (e.g. Bateson 1894; Cott 1940; Darwin 1871; Ford 1940; Huxley
1955). Color patterns provide a window to genetic variation, and thus provide a tool to
study evolutionary patterns and processes (Gulick 1873). Generally, color patterns are
thought to be adaptive, and function in thermoregulation, communication or predator
avoidance (Cott 1940; Endler 1978; Poulton 1890). When color patterns appear cryptic,
they are typically associated with predator avoidance (Owen 1980). Under selective
pressure of predators, multiple color patterns can be maintained in a population, if there is
temporal or spatial variation in predation, or a combination of both (Bond and Kamil
2006; Endler 1988; Haldane and Jayakar 1962; Hedrick et al. 1976).
One of the first mechanisms proposed to maintain color pattern polymorphism in
species with visual predators was frequency dependent predation (FDP) or apostatic
selection (Poulton 1884; Reviewed by Ayala and Campbell 1974). In FDP the fitness of a
morph depends on its frequency relative to other morphs in the population (Clarke and
O'Donald 1964), and is thus a mechanism of soft selection (Wallace 1975). FDP posits
that a visual predator will select the most common morph (Allen and Clarke 1984),
probably because development of a search image minimizes search time and thus
increases foraging efficiency (Allen 1988; Dukas and Kamil 2001; Hubbard et al. 1982;
60
Staddon and Gendron 1983; Tinbergen 1960). This decreases the chance of detection of
the alternative morphs, which will therefore increase their relative frequency within the
population, after which the same process repeats itself (Allen 1988; Ayala and Campbell
1974; Endler and Greenwood 1988; Fisher and Ford 1929). Frequency dependent
predation has been found to maintain polymorphism in a variety of cryptically colored
species that are preyed upon by visual predators such as birds or fish in natural
populations (Allen and Weale 2005; Olendorf et al. 2006; Reid 1987), as well as in
experiments with artificial prey (Allen and Clarke 1968; Bond and Kamil 1998; Bond
and Kamil 2002; Church et al. 1997; Fitzpatrick et al. 2009).
One of the main arguments against FDP concerns the process by which the
predator would have to select its prey items; mainly questioning the existence of search
image and continued switching behavior (Bond 2007; Punzalan et al. 2005). Experiments
have documented, however, that predators may indeed continue switching to the most
common prey item, even when familiarized with all prey types (Bond 1983; Bond and
Kamil 2002). Ultimately, the major interest in frequency dependent predation arose from
its role in maintaining color pattern polymorphism. Unless there is a frequency-dependent
effect on survival of the morphs (Endler 1986), frequency dependent predation is unlikely
to maintain multiple morphs in a population.
In anoles, several species have been documented to possess dorsal pattern
polymorphism in females (Chapter 2). In such cases, one female pattern may resemble
the male pattern, and one or two alternative dorsal patterns, usually a vertebral stripe or
diamond pattern, are found only in females (Fitch 1975; Savage 2002). Little is known
about the evolutionary basis for the occurrence of female polymorphism and only a few
61
studies have addressed female polymorphism in anoles. The dorsal patterns in females
are thought to have evolved in response to predation (Macedonia 2001; Schoener and
Schoener 1976; Chapter 3). Anoles are preyed upon by a variety of species, including
birds (Guyer 1988a; McLaughlin and Roughgarden 1989; Reagan 1996).
If alternative dorsal patterns in females evolved in response to predation, then
survival of females should be affected by their dorsal patterns. Here, we tested whether
frequency dependent predation could be maintaining female polymorphism in Norops
humilis, which would be supported by fluctuations in morph frequencies over time, and
lower survival of the most frequent morph.
METHODS
Study organism and data collection
Norops humilis (Peters 1863) is a small anole from lowland and pre-montane
moist forests and rainforests in Costa Rica and Panam?. There is some indication that N.
humilis may in fact represent distinct species among its geographic locations and the
population under study would become N. quaggulus (K?hler et al. 2006; K?hler et al.
2003). The division, however, is mostly based on hemi-penis morphology, and further
research is needed to determine whether this warrants a split of N. humilis. For the
purpose of this paper, we will use N. humilis sensu lato.
Males and females measure up to 45 mm snout-vent length (SVL) although
females average slightly larger than males (Fitch 1973; Fitch 1975). They perch low on
trunks and roam the leaf litter. Generations overlap and a single egg is laid every ten to
21 days throughout the year (Guyer 1988b). Juvenile males and females reach sexually
62
maturity after about four months, when they measure around 29 and 32 mm, respectively
(Guyer 1986; Talbot 1979b). Males have a brightly colored dewlap that is missing in
females. Lifespan of N. humilis is thought to be one year. Females of N. humilis can have
one of three morphs: a dotted pattern like males (?andromorph?), or one of two female
patterns (?gynomorph?): a tan to brown vertebral stripe or a reticulated pattern.
Norops humilis was studied between December 1982 and July 1983 at the La
Selva Biological Station in Costa Rica (Guyer 1988a; Guyer 1988b). The study site
consisted of three 15 X 15 m plots in former cacao/pejibaye/laurel plantations within a
premontane rainforest. N. humilis individuals were captured, toe-clipped and re-captured
during surveys with two to three day intervals between capture periods. At first capture,
individuals were measured to the nearest millimeter. The same site was surveyed for N.
humilis in 1988, 1993 and 2007, each time for several weeks.
Morph frequencies
Number of individuals per morph were calculated for both studies, separating
juvenile and adult females. Because sex determination in juveniles is difficult (Guyer
1988a), gender of some andromorph individuals was uncertain and juveniles included
were only those that lived long enough for the gender to be determined (1982-1983 data
only). Although observations occurred in multiple plots during all years, numbers were
too low for statistical analyses in all but the 1982-1983 data, and observations from
different plots were pooled. For the 1982-1983 dataset, data were presented per plot to
assess spatial variation in frequencies. Contingency tables were applied to the number of
individuals observed per morph for adult females of all years as well as for juvenile
females of 1982-1983.
63
Survival analysis
For the survival analysis, we used the long-term dataset of 1982-1983. The
capture history of that study consisted of 88 events at which an individual could be
captured (1) or not (0). Recapture of individuals was based on re-sighting, and thus
susceptible to missing individuals that were present. When an individual was not
captured, it could have been dead, or simply remained unseen. Because we used an open
population model, individuals that remained undetected, could have temporally or
permanently emigrated the plots. Survival analyses estimate recapture rates based on
individuals that were known to be present at a specific time but were not recaptured
during that time. Survival estimates are then calculated while controlling for recapture
probability (Lebreton et al 1992).
I tested for differences in survival between female morphs in juveniles and in
adults. I started with a saturated model (i.e., including all terms and interactions for both
survival and recapture) as recommended by Lebreton et al (1992). The live capture model
to investigate effects of dorsal patterns on female survival included effects of time (t),
pattern morph (m), age (a), and interactions between these variables on capture (p) and
survival probabilities (?). In this analysis time was not found to affect recapture
probability or survival, but statistics indicated that insufficient data were available to
estimate time effects. The analysis was repeated without this variable.
First, the saturated model was examined with a Goodness-of-Fit (GOF) test in the
program RELEASE (Burnham et al. 1987). The GOF test examines the underlying
assumptions that the probability of both recapture and survival of every marked
individual in the population is equal, regardless of their capture history. The saturated
64
model fitted the data for each test well (Table 1). Subsequent models were derived,
dropping one term at a time for recapture probability, and then comparing models using
Akaike?s information criterion (AICc) and likelihood ratio tests (Lebreton et al. 1992).
After modeling recapture rate, the same process was repeated for survival rates. Model
selection with logit transformation of variables was performed in the program MARK
version 5.0. for Windows (White and Burnham 1999).
RESULTS
Morph frequencies
Similar proportions of each morph were seen for juveniles and adults in the 1982-
1983 study: 50-60% andromorph females and 20-30% of each gynomorph pattern (Figure
1). In the 1982-1983 study, the relative proportion of each morph in the population did
not change from juveniles to adults (X
2
= 0.1873, d.f. = 2, P = 0.9106). Data recorded in
1988,1993 and 2007, resulted in a similar proportions of adult female morphs (Figure 1),
and no significant difference was found in morph frequency between years (X
2
= 2.4183,
d.f. = 6, P = 0.8775). Data were thus pooled to compare frequencies between morphs.
Andromorph females constituted a significantly higher proportion of female morphs in
juveniles X
2
juv
= 31.9644, d.f. = 2, P < 0.0001) as well as adults (X
2
ad
= 21.3849, d.f. = 2,
P < 0.0001).
Survival rates
I examined survival rates of females for effects of dorsal pattern and age, while
controlling for recapture rate. A GOF test showed that the fully parameterized Cormack-
Jolly-Seber model fit the data for all tests (Table 1). The model selection process for
65
effects of dorsal pattern in juveniles and adults on recapture and survival is detailed in
Table 2. From AICc values, we determined that recapture rate varied among dorsal
patterns for both juveniles and adults. In juveniles, recapture rate was lowest for the
dotted females (p = 0.31 + 0.01) and highest for the striped females (p = 0.41 + 0.02). In
adults, on the other hand, striped females had the lowest recapture rate (p = 0.16 + 0.03),
and the highest rate was seen in reticulated individuals (p = 0.32 + 0.03). Thus, effects of
morph and age on survival were modeled with recapture rate variation in age and morph.
Monthly survival probability was lower for juvenile than for adult females (?
(juvenile)
= 0.46 + 0.03, ?
(adult)
= 0.63 + 0.04, X
2
= 11.807, d.f. = 1, P = 0.0006). In
juveniles, monthly survival was lowest for reticulated and striped morphs and highest for
dotted ones (?
(dot)
= 0.47 + 0.04, ?
(retic)
= 0.44 + 0.06, ?
(stripe)
= 0.44 + 0.06), while in
adults survival was lowest for striped and similar for dotted and reticulated females (?
(dot)
= 0.65 + 0.05, ?
(retic)
= 0.65 + 0.10, ?
(stripe)
= 0.52 + 0.12). These differences,
however, were not significant; the model incorporating morph effects as well as age
effects did not differ significantly from the model incorporating only age effect on
survival probability (X
2
= 1.364, d.f. = 4, P = 0.8504).
DISCUSSION
We compared frequencies of female morphs between years, and tested for
differences in survival rates between female morphs to assess whether frequency
dependent selection could be maintaining female polymorphism in N. humilis at La
Selva. In all years, dotted females were the most common morph. Frequency dependent
predation should thus have resulted in lower survival of the dotted morph. Our data,
however, did not support this hypothesis. On the contrary, dotted morphs were found to
66
have slightly higher monthly survival rates than the other morphs in juveniles as well as
adults, although this difference was not significant.
The long-term nature of the 1982-1983 study along with comparable findings for
adults in three additional years by different observers warrants the conclusion that morph
frequencies are rather stable, and that they are not affected by observer bias (Rivera and
Andr?s 2001). The higher frequency of dotted morphs could also not be attributed to
morph specific differences in recapture rate, as these were not found to be higher for
dotted females compared to the other two morphs in the 1982-1983 dataset. As with
frequencies between years, morph frequencies did not appear to change from the juvenile
to the adult stage. Juvenile morph frequencies and survival should be approached with
some caution, because sex determination in juveniles is difficult, due to the matching
patterns of andromorph females and males. Both the constancy of the morph frequencies
and the similar survival rates of morphs, however, suggest that frequencies may indeed
remain stable from the juvenile to the adult stage.
Under the frequency dependent selection we expected fluctuations of frequencies
over time. Fluctuations can occur over very short or very long intervals due to, for
example, the predator?s sensitivity to changing frequencies of the morphs (Bond and
Kamil 1998; Merilaita 2006). Given the great similarity in morph frequencies over four
years, long-term fluctuations in these preliminary results seem unlikely. In addition,
survival was found not to differ significantly between morphs, thus agreeing with stable
frequencies over time.
Although frequency dependent predation is a commonly proposed mechanism to
explain color polymorphism, the complexity of food webs may reduce the chance for
67
frequency dependent predation to operate. Indeed, with an increased number of predators,
FDP is less likely to maintain polymorphism (Merilaita 2006), probably because of
differences in predator response to morph frequencies (Endler 1988). N. humilis is preyed
upon by a wide variety of predators (Guyer 1988a), and it was thus not surprising that
frequency-dependent predation was not supported by our results. Additionally, search
images for the most common morph are less likely to develop in heterogeneous
environments, or when predators prey on a wide variety of species (Kono et al. 1998).
Furthermore, survival is affected by factors other than predation. In N. humilis,
survival of adults increased when food was added to the habitat (Guyer 1988a),
suggesting that starvation affects mortality in this species. Similar to food limitations,
desiccation is also a possible cause of mortality in anoles (Hillman and Gorman 1977),
although this may be less important in our rainforest population of N. humilis. Disease
and parasites can affect survival too, although this seems to play a minor role in anoles
(Dobson et al. 1992; Schall and Pearson 2000). Because of the multiple factors affecting
mortality, survival analyses are not an ideal approach to study frequency dependent
predation (Van Gossum et al. 2004). Equal survival rates, however, suggest that
frequency dependent predation alone is not responsible for the occurrence of multiple
female dorsal patterns in our study population of N. humilis.
Similar survival rates of morphs in our study can thus result from several
scenarios. First, predation may actually differ between morphs, but is balanced by other
parameters affecting survival. In N. sagrei, for example, female morphs have been shown
to differ in immuno-competence (Calsbeek et al. 2008), and morphs may exert alternative
strategies to balance predation risk and immuno-competence. Second, morphs may
68
experience similar predation rate. This could result if there was no effect of morph on
predation. If morphs are selectively neutral, however, drift should lead to fixation of one
morph (Endler 1978). Similarly, morph frequencies could have reached equilibrium,
resulting in equal fitness (survival) of the morphs (Bond and Kamil 1998). When
selection results in equilibrium, drift can be compensated by rather weak selection
(Oxford 2005), which may be difficult to detect and experimental manipulation of morph
frequencies are required to test this hypothesis. Alternatively, equal survival probabilities
of female morphs could be achieved through morph-specific background matching
(Endler 1988; Hedrick et al. 1976; Stamps and Gon 1983).
69
Figure 1. Frequencies of female morphs observed. Top:. Observations of juvenile and
adult females in 1982-1983. Bottom:. Observations of adult females between 1982 and
2007. Dots (?),Reticulated (?), Stripe (*)
70
Table 1. Goodness of fit tests for saturated model examining effects of morph on survival
of juvenile and adult females of Norops humilis.
Category X
2
d.f. P
Juvenile dot 211.0486 198 0.250
Juvenile retic 62.8323 97 0.997
Juvenile stripe 69.1363 109 0.984
Adult dot 31.5678 104 1.0
Adult retic 5.092 53 1.0
Adult stripe 1.8724 23 1.0
TOTAL 381.5494 584 1.0
71
Table 2. Modeling recapture (P) and survival (?) as a function of age (a) and dorsal
pattern morph (m). The number of estimable parameters (np), deviance from the saturated
model (DEV) and the Akaike?s Information Criterion for small sample sizes (AICc) are
given for every model. LogLikelihood Ratio Tests were used to test for effects of time,
age and dorsal pattern, first on recapture rate and then on survival probability.
Model np DEV AICc Comparison
Saturated model:
1. ?m*a, Pm*a 12 5714.5 5949.5 Starting model fits the data
Modeling Capture Age effect on recapture:
2. ?m*a, pm 9 5782.0 6010.9 4 vs 3: X
2
= 60.871, d.f. = 1, P < 0.0001
3. ?m*a, pa 8 5745.1 5971.9 Morph effect on recapture:
4. ?m*a, p. 7 5805.9 6030.8 3 vs 1: X
2
= 30.527, d.f. = 4, P < 0.0001
Modeling Survival Morph effects on survival:
5. ?m, pm*a 9 5726.20 5955.0 6 vs 1: X
2
= 1.364 , d.f. = 4 , P = 0.8504
6. ?a, pm*a 8 5715.90 5942.7 Age effect on survival:
7. ?, pm*a 7 5727.70 5952.5 6 vs 7: X
2
= 11.807, d.f. = 1, P = 0.0006
72
CHAPTER FIVE
PREDATION ON FEMALES WITH ALTERNATIVE DORSAL PATTERNS IN
NOROPS HUMILIS
INTRODUCTION
In many animals, dorsal color patterns are associated with protection from
predation. Bright coloration in prey species often functions as a warning signal for the
predator, while drab color generally decreases visibility (Cott 1940; Edmunds 1974;
Poulton 1890). Perception of color patterns, however, depends on the background against
which the animal is seen (Bond 2007; Endler 1978; Ruxton et al. 2004), so that visibility
is not inherent to a color pattern. Reduced visibility to predators (camouflage) can
actually be obtained through color patterns that blend in with the background (crypsis) or
patterns that deflect the predator?s vision from the contours of its prey in which case the
colors need not be similar to the background (Cott 1940; Cuthill et al. 2007; Endler 1978;
Merilaita and Lind 2005). In a heterogeneous habitat, a camouflaging color pattern can be
the result of compromising for the variety of backgrounds to which an individual is
exposed; this pattern provides camouflage against a variety of backgrounds, but does not
maximize camouflage for each background separately (Merilaita et al. 2001; Merilaita et
al. 1999). Alternatively, individuals may select different microhabitats within the
heterogeneous environment and maximize camouflage for this particular microhabitat, so
73
that a variety of color patterns can be maintained within a population (Edmunds 1974;
Endler 1978; Endler 1984; Hedrick 1986). Therefore, predation in heterogeneous
environments may result in color polymorphic populations (Cain and Sheppard 1954b;
Ford 1945).
Anoles (Sauria: Iguanidae) are known to vary in dorsal patterns within
populations (e.g. Fitch 1975; Schoener and Schoener 1976). Moreover, these pattern
variations are mostly limited to females, and the same patterns re-occur in many species
(Savage 2002). The patterns vary in shape and do not appear to differ in color. A
common female-only morph is the cream-colored vertebral stripe. Another common
morph in female polymorphic species is a series of dorsal diamonds. The coloration
pattern of these anoles consists of varieties of brown and green, suggesting that these
patterns help reduce visibility to predators (Collette 1961; Fitch 1975; Macedonia 2001).
A comparative study found that female polymorphism in anoles is associated with
perch use (Chapter 3). Moreover, the vertebral stripe is thought to provide camouflage on
thin perches (Schoener and Schoener 1976). Previous studies indicated that female
morphs select different perches (Schoener and Schoener 1976; Steffen 2010). In
particular, striped females appear to select thinner perches in Norops sagrei (Schoener
and Schoener 1976) and higher perches in N. polylepis (Steffen 2010). These studies
were testing the hypothesis that dorsal pattern polymorphism in anoles is maintained
through alternative morphs selecting different backgrounds that provide the best
concealment for their particular pattern. Although perch use appears to differ between
female morphs, no study has addressed predation.
74
To determine visibility to predators of each pattern against different backgrounds,
experiments would be required. Methods have been developed to quantify how easily an
individual is seen by a predator depending on the background it is seen against (Endler
1990; Endler and Mielke 2005). While background matching is relatively easily
quantifiable for color polymorphism, such techniques have not been well developed for
pattern polymorphism (Endler and Mielke 2005). To test whether visibility of morphs to
predators is affected by perch type, I exposed modeling clay replicas of a common anole
species to free ranging predators. Clay models have gained popularity to assess the
interactions between predator and prey because predation is difficult to study under
natural circumstances (Bittner 2003; e.g. Brodie 1993; Howe et al. 2009; Noonan and
Comeault 2009; Steffen 2009). The advantage of clay models is that individual variation,
such as size and behavior, can be removed. Under the hypothesis of background
matching by morphs, I expected to find different predation rates for a variety of pattern-
background combinations. Specifically, I expected each morph to match at least one
background for which predation of this morph was lower than for other morphs. The
species under study, Norops humilis, has three morphs in females: vertebral dots, as in
males, a vertebral stripe or a series of diamonds. This species is found on the leaf litter
and low on trunks (Fitch 1975; Talbot 1979a). Backgrounds for this species can thus
consist of leaf litter and stems, and occasionally live leaves. Visibility of a pattern could
vary with the diameter of the stem or the height at which an individual perches. Based on
idea proposed by Schoener and Schoener (1976), I expected predation for striped morphs
to be lowest on thin perches. Considering the pattern in males is the dotted form, and
males perch higher than females (Fitch 1975; Talbot 1979a), I expected this pattern to
75
experience less predation on higher perches. Remaining perch sites for the reticulated
pattern are thus expected to be leaf litter and low perches with large diameter.
METHODS
Two experiments were carried out at the La Selva Biological Station, Costa Rica
(10?26?N, 83?59?W). Several species of anoles are found at La Selva that are
polymorphic in female dorsal patterns. The most common polymorphic species are
Norops humilis (sensu lato), N. limifrons and N. capito (Guyer and Donnelly 2004). N.
humilis measures up to 45 mm snout-vent length (SVL), and perches low on trunks and in
the leaf litter. N. limifrons perches somewhat higher and females grow to 43 mm SVL.
The larger N. capito measures up to 93 mm SVL and perches on tree trunks at heights
similar to N. limifrons (Fitch 1973; Fitch 1975; Talbot 1979a). Predators of these anoles
include spiders, birds, snakes, lizards and arthropods (Guyer 1988a). Females of the
polymorphic anoles at La Selva can have one of three dorsal patterns: a male-like pattern,
a reticulated pattern or a vertebral stripe that may vary from cream to brown depending
on weather conditions (Steffen, pers. comm.). The male-like pattern consists of vertebral
dots (sometimes no dots present) in N. humilis and N. limifrons. In N. capito, the entire
dorsum has a lichenous pattern.
Replicas were based on N. humilis, but were made slightly larger to allow
attachment to perches. Each model measured 50 mm SVL and the tails measured 70 mm,
which was still representative of polymorphic species of the study site. Replicas were
made from non-hardening, odorless, non-toxic modeling clay (VanAken
TM
), and
constructed with a self-made mould (SELVA brand silicone rubber HB). The soft clay
allowed observation of impressions resulting from predator attacks (Brodie 1993). The
76
brown base color for the models was chosen to match N. humilis by human eye. The clay
did not reflect UV, but UV radiation from the body has not been found in other species of
anoles and was therefore not expected to occur in N. humilis (Macedonia et al. 2003). The
three different dorsal patterns occurring in N. humilis were used in the models (Figure 1).
To limit the effect of the amount of cream color on the dorsum, all morphs, including the
dotted one, were based on a cream vertebral section. The outline of the reticulated pattern
and the vertebral stripe were drawn with a black permanent marker. The same marker
was also used for the vertebral dots and for the eyes. All models on perches were placed
head down and kept in place by a metal wire through the ventral midsection.
The first study took place from 8 June, 2007 to 26 June, 2007. Clay models were
placed on four different substrates that represented habitat elements where the
polymorphic anoles were seen: leaf litter, green (live) leaves, stems less than 2 cm
diameter, and stems more than 5 cm diameter. All stems were woody, to avoid effects of
green versus brown stems. On live leaves and stems, models were placed as close to 60
cm high as possible, because this is the average perch height of adult N. humilis (Talbot
1979a). Each pattern-substrate combination consisted of 44 models, for a total of 528.
Eleven study plots each contained four replications of each of twelve possible
combinations of dorsal pattern and substrate. A random number was assigned to every
combination using 'sample(48)' in R Version 2.8.1 (R Development Core Team 2008),
which was then used as the order in which the combinations were placed within the plot.
Models were placed in six rows. The minimum distance between models was two meters,
but this distance increased if the pre-determined perch type was located at a slightly
greater distance. Models were checked every third day. Models with predation were
77
removed from the plot and damaged models were repaired by smoothing the surface.
Because damage from ants increased over time, all models were collected after twelve
days.
In July 2007, a follow-up study further investigated predation on models placed
on stems that were chosen for perch height and diameter. Models were placed vertically
on stems of different diameters and at different heights. The diameter of the stem was
lower than 2 cm or thicker than 5 cm. Models were placed at 10 cm or 60 cm from the
forest floor. Combinations were randomized and placement depended on the availability
of perches as described above. For each of 12 combinations of pattern X diameter X
height, 50 replicas were prepared for a total of 600 models. They were placed in plots as
described above and left for four days.
Both experiments were carried out in disturbed forest (abandoned plantations,
secondary growth). All three common polymorphic species were encountered in this type
of habitat. Plots were separated by at least 100 m. Upon collecting the models, they were
placed in Ziploc bags and transported to the lab where predation marks were recorded.
Only V or U-shaped imprints and small symmetric slits and punctures were scored as
predation (Brodie 1993; Saporito et al. 2007; Steffen 2009) (Figure 2). Ant damage was
not scored as predation. When multiple predator marks were seen on one individual, a
single predation event was scored. Occasionally tails were found to have fallen off of the
model onto the ground. Marks on these tails were not included in the analysis. Models
that were not retrieved were considered lost. Analyses were run with and without scoring
the lost models as predation events.
78
In the first experiments, absence or presence of predation in the model was
compared for the different pattern-background combinations. I used a randomization test
of independence with 5000 iterations partitioned per background (McDonald 2009). This
test was also used to determine differences in predation at differing heights and on
differing diameters for each morph. A similar randomization test was also used to
determine differences in predation rate on morphs, independent of the background they
were on. All analyses were completed with proc freq in SAS/STAT ? software, Version
9.1.3 for SAS System for Windows.
RESULTS
Perch Substrate
Of 515 retrieved models, 111 (21%) showed signs of predation. Overall, predation
was highest on dotted models (resembling males and females), moderate for female-only
reticulated morphs, and lowest for female-only striped morphs (?
2
(2, N = 515)
= 9.0584, P =
0.0108). For models placed on live leaves, the number of predation events differed
significantly between morphs; striped morphs experienced less predation than expected,
while the opposite was true for the reticulated morph (?
2
(2, N = 129)
= 6.45, P = 0.0398;
Figure 3). On stems with a diameter less than two centimeters, the reticulated replica had
a lower attack rate and the dotted morph a much higher attack rate than expected (?
2
(2, N
= 131)
= 8.30, P = 0.0158). Predation in leaf litter was generally very low and did not
significantly differ among morphs (?
2
(2, N = 123)
= 0.81, P = 0.67). Similarly, no
significant difference was found in predator attacks on models placed on stems greater
than five centimeters in diameter (?
2
(2, N = 132)
= 4.26, P = 0.12). When repeating the
79
analysis with lost models scored as predation, results essentially remained the same (not
shown here).
Perch diameter and height
Of 600 models, 593 were retrieved and, of these, 6.7 % showed signs of
predation. No difference in predation was found for morphs placed at perches less than
two or more than five centimeters diameter (Dotted: ?
2
(1, N = 196)
= 0.88, P = 0.35;
Reticulated: ?
2
(1, N = 198)
= 0.07, P = 0.79; Striped: ?
2
(1, N = 199)
= 0.00, P = 0.95; Figure
4). When comparing predation between low and high perches, dotted and reticulated
morphs did not differ in predation (Dotted: ?
2
(1, N =196)
= 0.10, P = 0.76); Reticulated (?
2
(1, N = 198)
= 1.73, P = 0.19)). Striped morphs, on the other hand, were predated less on
lower than on higher perches (?
2
(1, N = 199)
= 4.95, P = 0.03). In this study, predation did
not differ between patterns (?
2
(1, N = 593)
= 0.66, P = 0.72). Repeating the analysis with
lost models did not change the conclusions.
DISCUSSION
The microhabitat hypothesis for maintaining female polymorphism predicts that
predation differs based on pattern-background combinations. Hence, survival would be
highest for females selecting the background that provides the best concealment for their
pattern. The prolonged survival and consequent higher lifetime reproductive output of
these females could be sufficient for polymorphism to be maintained. Our study showed
that clay replicas of Norops humilis differ in the rate of predator attacks based on the
substrate they were placed on. The best perches were low stems or green leaves for the
striped morph and thin high perches for the reticulated morph. For the dotted morph,
80
none of the perches under study resulted in lower predation compared to the other two
morphs.
The first study showed that the reticulated morph had a significant advantage
over striped and dotted morphs on thin stems, at least when placed at 60 cm high. When
low (10 cm) and high (60 cm) heights were combined in the second study, no effect of
diameter was seen on predation rates for the reticulated morph. The striped morph had
lower predation than expected on all substrates, but it had the advantage over the other
two on green leaves. Although the brown coloration of all morphs stands out against the
green background of these leaves, the stripe may function as a disruptive pattern (Cott
1940), rendering it more effective on backgrounds that are in strong contrast with the
brown body color (Stevens and Cuthill 2006). N. humilis, however, is rarely seen on
green leaves. Anoles are known to show a tight association between perch use and
morphological characters, such as body size and shape, limb length and number of toe
lamellae (Beuttell and Losos 1999; Losos 1990; Williams 1983). Morphology of N.
humilis may thus not be well-equipped to use live leaves as a perch. For stems, the
typical, elevated perch for N. humilis, striped morphs were found to experience
significantly less predation on low stems, compared to higher perches. Predation on the
dotted morph was generally higher than expected. The higher predation rate on the dotted
morph in the first study suggests that this pattern may be the least concealing.
In the second study, no such difference was observed, but this could have resulted
from the lower predation rate in the second study. Predation in the first study was high
compared to the typical predation rate of 5-10% on clay models (Bittner 2003; Husak et
al. 2006). This rate was found in studies leaving models in the field between four and
81
seven days and thus the difference in predation rate may be due to the relatively long time
period (twelve days) during which models in my first study were in the field. Indeed,
predation rate per day was 1.75%, so that four days would result in a 7% predation rate.
The second study, in which models were left out for four days, predation rate was 6.7%,
and thus consistent with predation rates generally seen in clay model studies.
A higher predation on the dotted morph is surprising, considering it is the typical
male pattern and the most common morph in females of N. humulis (Chapter 4). Males
and females, however, differ in several characteristics and the patterns may affect
predation differently on males and females. For example, males may not benefit from a
protective pattern, at least not as an adult, because their visibility is increased through
displays of their colorful dewlaps (e.g. Fleishman 1991). Males and females may also
differ in the shape of their abdomen due to the eggs in the continuously gravid females,
and this could lead to different visual effect from the dorsal patterns (Merilaita and Lind
2005). Indeed, a stripe pattern has been shown to reduce predation in gravid females, but
not in other females or males in an Australian skink, Lampropholis delicata (Forsman
and Shine 1995). The clay models in my study did not differ in shape and it would be
interesting to test whether a bulging abdomen would reduce predation rates on female-
only morphs even further. Alternatively, males may rely more on flight and females on
camouflage, for example if movement of females is impeded by the presence of eggs
(Bauwens and Thoen 1981; Lailvaux et al. 2003; Lee et al. 1996; but see Schwarzkopf
and Shine 1992).
Furthermore, visibility of patterns is affected by movement (Stevens 2007). Thus,
the variation in predation on the different morphs in this study is only applicable to
82
motionless individuals. But, dotted morphs are expected to experience higher predation
than the other morphs when moving, because the dots provide fixed reference points for
predators (Brown 1931a; Brown 1931b). Dots are therefore associated with anti-predator
techniques that do not rely on movement, such as remaining motionless or aggressive
displays (Brodie 1992). This could explain why male anoles have the dotted pattern;
dewlap displays may be used to deter predators (Leal and Rodriguez-Robles 1997). It
does not, however, explain why the majority of females is dotted (Chapter 4). Perhaps,
the high frequency of dotted females is the result of a dominant allele coding for vertebral
dots. In female polymorphic damselflies, the pattern seen in males as well as females was
found to be coded by a dominant autosomal allele (Andres and Cordero 1999).
Alternatively, dotted females could benefit in other ways; for example a benefit might
accrue through increased male attention based on their higher frequency in the population
(Cordero 1992; van Gossum et al. 2001).
Females of N. humilis spend much time on the leaf litter and predation was low in
the leaf litter for all morphs. The lower predation in the leaf litter could be because the
different morphs are equally camouflaged in this diverse background. Alternatively,
predation on the leaf litter could be lower due to an effect of height on predation rates.
Anoles have been shown to experience lower predation at lower perch heights (Steffen
2009). This was confirmed by results of my second study, where all morphs experienced
less predation on low versus high perches, although this was only significant for the
striped morph. Lower predation on females in the leaf litter could result in relaxed
selection on morphs. Because not all female polymorphic anoles frequently roam the leaf
litter, this could not explain the occurrence of multiple morphs in those species.
83
Overall, my results supported the hypothesis of differential predation on morphs
based on the background against which they were seen, but findings were not consistent
with my predictions, which were based on perch use observations of female morphs in
two other species that did not occur at my study site. The question arises if morphs of N.
humilis at La Selva prefer the perch where they experience less predation.
84
Figure 1. Clay models. A. From top to bottom: dotted, striped and reticulated
pattern. B. Model on leaf litter, C. Model attached to perch.
A
B C
85
Figure 2. Bite marks categorized as predation.
86
Figure 3. Predation frequencies for pattern-background combinations from experiment 1.
Each background is represented separately. Significant difference at 0.05 level are
indicated with a star (*). Both observed and expected frequencies are shown to
demonstrate how the patterns differed.
87
Figure 4. Predation for different heights and diameters of woody stems from experiment
2. Significance is indicated by a star (*). Sample sizes are given above the bars.
88
CHAPTER SIX
FEMALE DORSAL PATTERN POLYMORPHISM IN NOROPS HUMILIS
(Squamata: Polychrotidae): PERCH USE AND A TEST OF THE
MICROHABITAT HYPOTHESIS.
INTRODUCTION
Intra-specific variation provides the evolutionary potential for evolution and
speciation (Gray and McKinnon 2007). Research on mechanisms for maintaining intra-
specific genetic variation in color and color patterns has led to a better understanding of
how evolution proceeds in an environmental context. Classic examples in the study of
color pattern polymorphism are Cepaea snails and the peppered moth Biston betularia, in
which color morphs were associated with background matching for predator avoidance
and thermoregulation in changing habitats (See Cook 1998; Cook 2003; Jones et al.
1977). They became important examples to elucidate the role of spatially and temporally
heterogeneous environments in maintaining polymorphism. Theoretical approaches of
how polymorphism can persist in a heterogeneous environment were introduced by
Levene (1953). Elaborations on his work demonstrated that particularly spatial variation
plays an important role in the maintenance of genetic polymorphism (Reviewed by
Hedrick 1986; Hedrick et al. 1976).
Polymorphism under spatial variation is often related to cryptic color
polymorphism, where alternative morphs choose specific microhabitats to reduce
89
visibility to predators (e.g., Garciadorado 1986; Kettlewell and Conn 1977; Morey 1990;
Sandoval 1994). Because microhabitat choice as well as predation risk may differ
between males and females (Pocklington and Dill 1995; Slatkin 1984), the occurrence of
multiple morphs may be restricted to one gender (Forsman and Shine 1995; Lynch et al.
1985; Merilaita and Jormalainen 1997). The microhabitat hypothesis for the maintenance
of polymorphism in only one gender states that the polymorphic gender chooses
backgrounds that optimize concealment of their color pattern, while the other gender may
be using a smaller range of micro-habitats, thus restricting their options for concealing
patterns (Stamps and Gon 1983).
In anoles (Squamata: Polychrotidae), a group of Neotropical lizards, females of
many species vary in dorsal patterns, especially among mainland species (e.g., Fitch
1975; Chapter 2; Schoener and Schoener 1976). Most anoles are colored in variations of
brown or green, with some blue in a few species. Dorsal patterns can consist of transverse
bands, multiple longitudinal lines, indistinct patterns such as vermiculations or a
lichenous pattern, or uniform coloration without any clear pattern. In addition, these
dorsal color patterns may or may not incorporate one of the following middorsal patterns:
blotches, a cream-to-orange-colored stripe with or without dark outlining, reticulated or
diamond shapes or black X shapes resulting in a diamond-like pattern, or a herringbone
pattern (Savage 2002; Chapter 3). These middorsal patterns consist of the same drab
colors that are seen on the rest of their body. All middorsal patterns consist of similar
colors. The middorsal stripe and the diamond pattern are often formed by a cream-
colored tint of brown that is paler compared to dominant tints on the rest of the body.
90
The dorsal patterns have been shown to be heritable in one female polymorphic
species, Norops sagrei (Calsbeek et al. 2008). Patterns are already clearly expressed in
hatchlings, and are maintained throughout life. Females may be ?andromorph? (i.e. male-
like), and an additional one or two ?gynomorph? (i.e. only in females) patterns occur
(Fitch 1975; Guyer 1988a). Moreover, very similar patterns are recognized across species
with roughly five categories of middorsal patterns and generally two or three varieties per
population (Savage 2002). These five female middorsal patterns are mostly variations of
the middorsal stripe and the diamond-like patterns described above. Overall, females
differ only in dorsal pattern and not in color, as is more common in other species with
female-limited color pattern polymorphism. The drab coloration of the patterns in female
polymorphic anoles hints at the possibility for a link between the dorsal patterns and
predator avoidance (Endler 1978). Furthermore, female polymorphism is most common
among species that use trunks as one of their main perch sites (Chapter 3), indicating that
perching behavior may play an important role in the maintenance of FPP. Indeed,
relationships between morph and perch use have been found in one island and one
mainland anole species (Schoener and Schoener 1976; Steffen 2010). The occurrence of
spatial variation in predation based on combinations of pattern and perch characteristics
was demonstrated with a study using clay models representing Norops humilis (Chapter
5). Studying perch use of this species in the same environment would provide more
conclusive results regarding the role of predation and micro-habitat use in female
polymorphism.
We investigated the microhabitat hypothesis of Stamps and Gon (1983) as an
explanation for the maintenance of female polymorphism in a mainland anole, Norops
91
humilis. This hypothesis predicts that females should perch at different heights.
Specifically, we predicted that striped morphs would perch lower than reticulated morphs
based on the differences in predation experienced by these morphs (Chapter 5). In
relation to the limitation of polymorphism to females, the micro-habitat hypothesis states
that females should use a wider spectrum of microhabitats than males. Hence, we
expected to see greater variability of perch heights in females compared to males.
METHODS
Study organism
Norops humilis (Peters 1863, sensu lato) occurs throughout the lowlands of
Panam? and Costa Rica. N. humilis perches low on trunks and on the ground. Juveniles
roam the forest floor, while adults perch up to 200 cm high on stems (Fitch 1975; Talbot
1979b). Males and females are reproductive year round (Fitch 1973; Guyer 1986). The
adult sex ratio is slightly male biased, and varies throughout the year (Guyer 1988a).
Adulthood is thought to be reached around four to six months of age, when males have
reached a snout-vent-length (SVL) of ca. 29 mm and females measure ca. 32 mm (Fitch
1970; Fitch 1973; Guyer 1986; Talbot 1979b). Individuals of N. humilis are thought to
live up to one year.
Data collection
We used data from two separate studies of N. humilis at the La Selva Biological
station in Costa Rica (10?26?N, 83?59?W). The first study was carried out in 1982-1983,
the second one in 2007. Both studies observed individuals of N. humilis in abandoned
plantations (cacao, pejibaye, laurel). At La Selva, female N. humilis exhibit three morphs
92
within the population: a dotted andromorph, and striped and reticulated gynomorph
patterns.
The 1982-1983 study consisted of capture-recapture data on six plots, of which
three were supplemented with food for a different study (Guyer 1988a; Guyer 1988b).
Only individuals from the three non-food-supplemented plots in this dataset were
included. Individuals in this study were toe-clipped for identification. At each capture
occasion, SVL, tail condition, perch height, perch substrate, perch location, gender (when
possible), age category (juvenile/adult) and dorsal pattern were recorded. Individuals with
missing data were omitted. In the 2007 study, six plots were surveyed three times over
the course of three weeks in May. Individuals were not marked. The same variables were
recorded as in the first study.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed with the software package R Version 2.8.1 (R Development
Core Team 2008). Perch height data were not normally distributed for either dataset,
mainly due to the long tail caused by the many leaf litter observations. Therefore, we
used a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test for the perch data from the 2007 study in which
individuals were not marked and observations were assumed to be independent. In the
1982-1983 study, perch height of each (marked) individual was recorded at every
observation and not all individuals were seen an equal number of times. Therefore,
observations were not independent and could not be averaged over the number of
observations. To account for varying numbers of repeated measurements per individual,
this dataset was analyzed with generalized estimating equations (GEE) in the R-package
?geepack? (Yan 2002; Yan and Fine 2004). In GEE, distribution type is not a major
93
influence and varying the distribution between normal and Poisson did not lead to
different results. Results shown are for the Poisson distribution. For model selection, the
Wald test was used (Zuur et al. 2009). The full model accounted for all possible variation
and included the variables pattern, age, plot and gender, plus all interactions among these
variables with the exception of the gender-pattern interaction. This interaction was
irrelevant considering males only have one pattern (Table 1). Data were sorted based on
date of observation per individual. The ?exchangeable? correlation structure was used
because there was no temporal factor; the correlation structure would be caused by
individual perch height preference (Hardin and Hilbe 2003). To determine variation in
microhabitat use for males and females, we applied a Levene test to perch heights to test
whether deviations from the mean were larger for males than for females. The Levene
test was carried out in the package ?car? (Fox 2009). Next, we tested for a difference in
elevated perch use. Many of the observations on N. humilis were on the leaf litter. These
observations were removed from the larger 1982-1983 dataset, and analyses of perch
height differences between morphs and perch height variance for males and females were
repeated.
RESULTS
Perch height with leaf litter observations
Perch height analysis from the 1982-1983 study showed that perch height was
determined by age, gender, plot and the interaction between gender and plot and between
gender and age (Table 1). Pattern or interactions with pattern were not significant. The
estimated correlation parameter indicating individual preference for perch heights was
94
low (alpha = 0.0858, S.E. = 0.0172). Perch height averages are shown in Table 2.
Juveniles tended to perch lower than adults and males perched consistently higher than
females in both juveniles and adults (Table 2). The 2007 study showed very similar
results. Perch height of adult females did not differ significantly among morphs (X
2
(df=2)
=
3.534, N = 27, P = 0.172), but adult females perched lower than adult males (X
2
(df=1)
=
10.3, N = 60, P = 0.0013). Perch height also differed between adults and juveniles
(X
2
(df=1)
= 12.6, N = 90, P < 0.001).
Next, we tested for a difference in variance in perch height between males and
females to determine whether females showed a higher variability in perch height than
males. In the 1982-1983 study the maximum recorded perch height for males was 200
cm, for females 130 cm. For female adults, much variation was seen among the three
plots. Variation in average perch height for juvenile females appeared much lower. The
Levene test for homogeneity of variance showed that male and female adults differed
significantly in variance of perch height in this study (F
(1,1182)
= 48.903, P < 0.0001,
S.D.
males
= 29.6 cm, S.D.
females
= 20.1 cm). Maximum recorded perch height for adult
males and female in the 2007 study was 81 cm and 70 cm, respectively. No difference in
perch height variance was found between males and females in this more limited sample
of observations (F
(1,58)
= 1.98, P = 0.164, S.D.
males
= 21.9 cm, S.D.
females
= 18.4 cm).
Perch height without leaf litter observations
Juveniles as well as adults were often observed on the forest floor. In the 1982-
1983 dataset, 70.8% and 64.3% of observations of juvenile females and males,
respectively, were from the leaf litter. For adult females, leaf litter observations
constituted 67.7% of the total, while for males this was only 45.8%. Observation of
95
female morphs on the forest floor were very similar: 69%, 68% and 64% for dotted,
reticulated and striped females, respectively. The analysis was repeated on a dataset
without these leaf litter observations.
We found pattern, gender, age and plot as well as most interactions between these
variables to explain perch height (Table 3). Although juveniles spent more time on the
forest floor than adults, removal of these observations still led to lower perch height of
juveniles compared to adults (P
age
< 0.0001; Table 4). Males were found to sit higher on
elevated perches than females (P
gender
< 0.0001). For the three female morphs, perch
height was variable among plots. Striped females were found to perch higher than dotted
or reticulated females in all plots (P
pattern
= 0.0002). Dotted females perched lower than
reticulated females, except in the plot with only one reticulated female on an elevated
perch. Variance in perch height of adult males and females on elevated perches alone did
not differ significantly ( F
(1,550)
= 1.42, P = 0.235, S.D.
males
= 31.0 cm, S.D.
females
= 25.5
cm)
DISCUSSION
In heterogeneous habitats, dorsal patterns might be optimized to match a variety
of patches within the habitat (Merilaita et al. 2001; Merilaita et al. 1999), or they might
be optimized for a specific background, so that multiple morphs co-exist in a population
(Edmunds 1974; Endler 1978; Endler 1980). In the latter case, individuals are expected to
select micro-habitat patches that best fit their pattern to achieve concealment from
predators (Edmunds 1974; Endler 1978; Endler 1984; Sandoval 1994). If males are more
restricted in micro-habitat use than females are and dorsal patterns are related to micro-
habitat use, then polymorphism should be more likely to evolve in females. This forms
96
the micro-habitat hypothesis for the maintenance of female polymorphism (Stamps and
Gon 1983).
We tested the micro-habitat hypothesis for Norops humilis, a low perching anole
of Central America. We expected the three morphs observed in females to differ in perch
height. Moreover, males were expected to use a narrower range of perch heights than
females. We did not find a difference in perch height between female morphs when
including leaf litter observations in perch use. In our analysis of elevated perches only,
we found a significant difference in perch height among morphs. There were also
interaction effects of pattern with grid and with age. All juvenile females appeared to
perch at similar heights, regardless of pattern. Within adults, dotted females perched
lowest and striped females consistently perched higher, on average, than the other two
morphs. In another female polymorphic mainland anole, N. polylepis, striped females
were also found to perch higher than other morphs (Steffen 2010). The finding that
striped females perch higher than other morphs contradicts our prediction that striped
females should be perching lower than the other two morphs to reduce predation (Chapter
5).
The vertebral stripe is a common female morph in many of the female
polymorphic anoles is a cream-colored vertebral stripe, sometimes bordered by black
(Savage 2002). This pattern may be beneficial to conceal females from predators as a
disruptive pattern (Cott 1940). Perch diameter could therefore be an important factor in
concealment, but a relationship between the presence of a vertebral stripe and perch
diameter has found support in only one study on female polymorphic anoles and not in
others (Schoener and Schoener 1976; See also Calsbeek et al., 2008; Steffen, 2010). In
97
addition, a study of predation on female morphs at our study site found differences in
predation rate based on perch height but not diameter (Chapter 5). Perch height is thus
appropriate to test the microhabitat hypothesis and our results thus suggest that the this
hypothesis may not explain the occurrence of multiple morphs in females of N. humilis at
La Selva.
This conclusion is further supported by the great variation in perch use. Data in
the 1982-1983 study indicated overdispersion, caused by large variation in perch height
use. This could be expected if perch heights were random, because without restrictions to
perch use, individuals are expected to move over the entire range of possible heights. In
addition, perch height distribution was zero-inflated due to the large number of
observations of individuals on the forest floor. Because possible perch heights for N.
humilis range from 0 to about 200 cm high, and because more surface area is available on
the leaf litter than on perches, the null-model predicts such a skew in perch height
distribution. Furthermore, no individual preference for perch height was detected (very
low alpha value in GEE). Also within morphs perch height was highly variable, even if
leaf litter observations were not considered (Table 4). The high variation in perch use, the
rather small differences in average perch heights between morphs and low individual
perch height preference lead us to think that any perch height differences observed
between females are unlikely related to concealment from predators. These results thus
refute the micro-habitat hypothesis.
Stamps and Gon (1983) stated that the micro-habitat hypothesis can be refuted if
males vary more in habitat use than females. Although our data showed that variance in
perch height is indeed higher for males than for females of N. humilis, this need not be a
98
prediction of the microhabitat hypothesis. Males and females often differ behaviorally
and morphologically, and the lack of polymorphism in male dorsal patterns could be
attributed to these differences. For example, an experiment in a scincid lizard showed that
the striped female-only pattern did not increase male survival, but it increased survival of
gravid females (Forsman and Shine 1995), leading the authors to suggest that body shape
differences may affect visibility of the pattern to predators. Alternatively, female mate
choice could limit variation in males, as is seen in female polymorphic butterflies (Krebs
and West 1988).
In spite of the great variation in perch use, we found that males perched higher
than females, and adults perched higher than juveniles. Similar patterns have been found
for a variety of low perching species, including N. humilis (Fitch 1975; Talbot 1979b).
Males and females have been shown to be adapted to different microhabitats (Butler
2007; Butler and Losos 2002), which could be attributed to differing resource needs.
Males may perch higher to focus more on females as a resource, while females may focus
more on food acquisition for reproduction (Andrews 1971; Davies 1991; Fitch 1975;
Guyer 1988b; Guyer 1994; Parmelee and Guyer 1995; Perry 1996; Scott et al. 1976;
Talbot 1979b). Resource access could also explain the lower perch height of juveniles.
Availability of prey of suitable size for juveniles may be higher near the leaf litter. At the
same time, juveniles still need to establish their home range and territory, and are
confined mostly to the forest floor until they are large enough to do so. Perch height
difference between morphs were much smaller than between the age and gender groups,
and there is no clearly emerging relationship with resource use.
99
Inconspicuous color patterns are often related to predator avoidance (Endler 1978;
Merilaita and Jormalainen 1997). The multiple dorsal patterns in female anoles were thus
expected to be the result of morph-specific predation. More specifically, we expected
morphs to choose microhabitats that would lower their chance of predation. Although we
found a slight difference in perch height between morphs, these results were not
consistent with predator avoidance. Another predator driven mechanism for maintaining
polymorphism is negative frequency dependent predation, where the predator continues
to switch his search image to the most common morph (Allen 1988; Ayala and Campbell
1974). The constancy of morph frequencies and similar survival rates of the female
morphs in the juvenile, as well as in the adult stage, however, refute this alternative
predator-based hypothesis (Chapter 4). We can thus conclude that multiple dorsal
patterns in a population of Norops humilis are not maintained by predation, at least not by
predation alone.
100
Figure 1. Female morphs occurring in Norops humilis in the population of La Selva
Biological Station, Costa Rica. A. The andromorph dotted pattern, B. the gynomorph
reticulated or diamond pattern, C. the gynomorph striped pattern.
A B C
101
Table 1. Model selection steps. Starting with the full model, one term (and any
interactions including this term) was dropped. A Wald test was used to compare the
models. A new model was then built with all terms except for the least significant one
and the process was repeated until all terms were significant (?Final Model?). The process
was repeated once more to ensure that all terms were significant. The degrees of freedom
(Df), Chi Square value from the Wald test (X2) and P-levels from the Wald test are
provided. The estimated scale parameter (Est. Scale Par.) and estimated correlation
parameter (Est. Corr. Par.) are provided.
Full Model:
Perch ~ Pattern * Age * Plot + Gender:Age + Gender:Plot +
Gender+Gender:Age:Plot
Least significant term
dropped: Df X
2
P
Age:Gender:Grid 2 1.3914 0.4987
Age:Pattern:Grid 4 9.1900 0.0565
Age:Pattern 2 1.1246 0.5699
Age:Plot 2 2.4307 0.2966
Pattern:Plot 4 5.7495 0.2187
Pattern 2 4.4468 0.1082
Final Model: Perch~Age+Plot+Gender+Gender:Plot+Gender:Age+Gender:Plot
Age 2 31.179 <0.0001
Estimate S.E.
Plot 4 26.618 <0.0001 Est. Scale Par. 34.31 1.956
Gender 4 43.898 <0.0001 Est. Corr. Par. 0.0858 0.01716
Gender:Age 3 13.8577 0.0031 Number of clusters: 499
Gender:Plot 2 12.6217 0.0018
102
Table 2. Average perch heights. Perch heights were first averaged per individual, then per
category. A. Results from the 1982-1983 study per grid. Heights (in centimeters) are
provided for each of the categories (grid, age, and pattern). B. Results from the 2007
study. Andro: the male pattern, Stripe: the middorsal stripe in females, Retic: the
reticulated middorsal pattern in females. Standard deviation (S.D.) and sample size (n)
are provided below the mean values.
Gender/ Age: Female Juvenile Female Adult
Male
Juvenile
Male
Adult
Perch
Height
(cm) Grid Pattern Andro Retic Stripe Andro Retic Stripe Andro Andro
Mean
S.D. 1
N
4.6
7.8
( 50)
17.6
27.9
(12)
7.0
9.9
(10)
5.0
9.2
(11)
16.4
20.9
(6)
18
n/a
(1)
19.1
21.2
(45)
25.8
27.8
(53)
Mean
S.D. 2
N
9.6
22.7
(48)
5.1
8.7
(21)
4.4
6.2
(20)
16.3
25.1
(22)
16.1
24.2
(7)
14.6
22
(11)
8.3
10.6
(45)
16.9
19.3
(48)
Mean
S.D.
A
3
N
5.9
9.9
(36)
7.2
7.0
(13)
5.2
9.5
(14)
8.4
13.5
(16)
16.1
21.1
(3)
7.1
6.7
(5)
8.0
14.3
(41)
17.1
19.1
(43)
Mean
S.D. B
N
6.9
6.7
(7)
0.6
1.5
(7)
6
8.5
(2)
9.5
6.7
(13)
21.3
1.5
(6)
11.8
8.5
(8)
8.89
11.6 (14)
29.4
21.9
(33)
103
Table 3. Model selection steps without leaf litter data. For further details, see text and
Table 1.
Full Model:
Perch ~ Pattern * Age * Plot + Gender:Age + Gender:Plot +
Gender+Gender:Age:Plot
Least significant term dropped: Df X2 P
Age:Gender:Grid 2 2.72 0.2566
Final Model:
Age 10 55.4 2.66.10
-8
Estimate S.E.
Plot 14 57 3.84.10
-7
Est. Scale Par. 18.0 1.63
Gender 8 43.3 7.70.10
-7
Est. Corr. Par. 0.0749 0.0312
Gender:Age 3 8.68 0.0339 Number of clusters: 318
Age:Pattern:Grid 4 13.0 0.0113
Age:Pattern 6 19.1 0.0041
Age:Plot 6 20.3 0.0025
Pattern:Plot 8 17.83 0.0225
Pattern 12 37.5 0.0002
Gender:Plot 2 8.51 0.0142
104
Table 4. Average perch heights and standard deviations for 1982-1983 dataset without
leaf litter observations.
Gender/ Age: Female Juvenile Female Adult
Male
Juvenile
Male
Adult
Perch
Height (cm)
Grid Pattern Andro Retic Stripe Andro Retic Stripe Andro Andro
Mean
S.D. 1
N
21.4
12.7
(51)
26.3
13.6
(16)
25.7
18.5
(23)
30.0
31.6
(7)
20.0
n/a
(1)
43.3
29.4
(6)
37.0
26.7
(97)
46.7
38.1
(133)
Mean
S.D. 2
N
23.0
22.0
(87)
27.3
16.2
(41)
21.0
12.2
(31)
27.2
26.1
(44)
41.2
22.9
(21)
67.0
38.7
(5)
23.4
19.1
(97)
31.2
24.1
(178)
Mean
S.D.
3
N
21.1
15.1
(39)
21.8
14.0
(11)
24.5
14.7
(39)
20.9
12.0
(23)
26.9
22.2
(8)
30.0
10.0
(3)
23.5
18.2
(70)
37.5
29.2
(123)
105
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
The abundance of polymorphism in nature poses an important problem for
evolutionary biologists, because theory predicts minimal variation through the
evolutionary mechanisms of drift or directional selection (Futuyma 1998). Because color
patterns are easily observable, they provide an excellent tool for studying polymorphism.
In spite of the large body of research on this topic, female polymorphism has received
comparatively little attention and what studies there are focused mostlyon invertebrates.
In a brief review on female polymorphism in my introductory chapter, I proposed that
anoline lizards are an excellent model system to study female color polymorphism in
vertebrates.
Chapter two revealed that female polymorphism has originated independently in
several ancestors and in some single species, contrary to the well-studied damselflies
where female polymorphism is an ancestral trait (Futuyma 1998; Van Gossum and
Mattern 2008). Furthermore, several independent losses were observed. Thus, the study
of anoles allows addressing both the origination and maintenance of female
polymorphism. These results also provide support for the idea of phylogenetic inheritance
of genetic variation (as opposed to inheritance of an individual trait). That is, variation at
the population level can persist throughout the speciation process, occurring in both the
ancestral and descendent species (Jose et al. 2008). This has implications for future
research on female polymorphism in anoles; comparative analyses should incorporate
106
phylogeny to account for phylogenetic signal in this trait (Felsenstein 1985; Freckleton et
al. 2002). The pattern of phylogenetic inheritance of female polymorphism only occurred
within geographically isolated areas, suggesting that geographic isolation may have been
a prerequisite to the pattern of independent origination of female polymorphism in
anoles. The Lesser Antilles formed the exception; female polymorphism likely resulted
from a female polymorphic ancestor for each of two independent radiations inhabiting
these islands, even though the different species inhabit separate islands. I hypothesized
that the closer distance of these islands may have prevented loss of variation, but this
needs to be tested.
The comparative study in the next chapter indicated that losses of female
polymorphism in the norops clade of the mainland were associated with changes in perch
use. This association was weaker for other clades, but analyses indicated that female
polymorphism was more likely to evolve in species with certain habitat use patterns,
particularly trunk perching anoles and species inhabiting a variety of different habitat
types. The reason for these associations needs further investigation. An association with
habitat was expected under an adaptive hypothesis, because visual signals, such as dorsal
patterns, are affected by the light environment (Endler 1993). I addressed one of the
possible adaptive hypotheses associated with habitat: female polymorphism is maintained
through predation.
Species where female polymorphism of dorsal patterns (FPP) is the result of
ancestral evolution, such as Norops humilis, are ideal to study maintenance (as opposed
to origination) of FPP. In spite of the intuitive appeal to associate the drab and perhaps
cryptic dorsal patterns with predation, my study could not support the hypothesis that
107
female polymorphism in Norops humilis at La Selva was maintained by predation. The
commonly invoked mechanism of frequency dependent predation could maintain
polymorphism if predators continuously switch to the most common morphs, so that
fitness is inversely related to the frequency of a morph (Ayala and Campbell 1974).
Equal survival rates of morphs and similar morph frequencies in space and time refuted
the hypothesis of frequency dependent predation in Chapter four. Equal survival rates
would be expected, however, under the micro-habitat hypothesis. This theorizes that
polymorphism can be maintained if alternative morphs differ in their visibility based on
the background they are seen against, and anoles should thus choose perches that
minimized predation (Hedrick et al. 1976; Stamps and Gon 1983). However, the
differences in perch use I observed contradicted predictions based on predator avoidance
determined by a predation study in Chapter five.
Anoles are preyed upon by a variety of predators, undoubtedly resulting in
various predation techniques. The effectiveness of camouflage patterns thus depends on
the relative importance of visually oriented predators. For N. humilis, observations
suggested that ctenid spiders may be the most important predator (Guyer 1988a), but
predation events by birds may just be more difficult to encounter. Ctenid spiders appear
to have great visual capabilities including color vision and shape detection (Barth et al.
1993; Land and Barth 1992; Schmid 1998; Walla et al. 2009), although their ability to
distinguish between dorsal patterns remains unclear. Drab dorsal color patterns may in
fact counter selection pressure from predators, but without contributing to the
maintenance of female color polymorphism. In other words, polymorphism might be
108
influenced by other selection pressures, while predation itself simply ensures that all
patterns will be drab.
Female color patterns could thus be a compromise between camouflage and
another function (Endler 1978; Endler 1980; Merilaita et al. 2001). Besides serving as a
predator avoidance mechanism, color patterns may function in thermoregulation and as a
signal in communication such as attracting mates (Cott 1940; Endler 1978).
Thermoregulation seems unlikely, because N. humilis is a thermo-conformer (i.e. no
behavioral regulation of temperature) (Fitch 1973; Fitch 1975). A function in attracting
mates seems unlikely given that females can probably store sperm (Fox 1963; Jeffrey and
David 1980; Sever and Hamlett 2002), and thus limiting the need of acquiring multiple
matings. Although the presence of eggs has not been investigated in relation to dorsal
pattern, adult females are almost invariably found gravid (Fitch 1973; Guyer 1986).
Genetic diversity of offspring increases with matings in anoles (Calsbeek et al. 2007),
and attracting mates could therefore increase reproductive success through sperm
competition, for example through increased genetic diversity of offspring (Jennions and
Petrie 2000; Keller and Reeve 1995). But, there is little evidence for mate choice in
anoles (Andrews 1985; Tokarz 1995; Tokarz 1998), and with the colorful dewlap
displays of males, females rather than males are expected to choose (Stamps 1983; West-
Eberhard 1983). This is further supported by a male-skewed adult sex-ratio in the
population of N. humilis in this study (Guyer 1988a; Guyer 1994; Kvarnemo and Ahnesjo
1996). Alternative selective hypotheses for the maintenance of female polymorphism in
anoles thus seem difficult to generate. Some similarities between female polymorphic
species and differences between morphs could guide future research.
109
The comparative study of Chapter three showed that evolution of female
polymorphism was associated with habitat use, in particular perch use. An association
with perch use was further supported by the differences in perch use between female
morphs in a population study of N. humilis addressed in Chapter six. Females differed
slightly, but significantly in the use of elevated perches, with adult striped females
perching highest and dotted females perching lowest in all plots. This finding was
consistent with findings in another female polymorphic mainland anole (Steffen 2010).
Differences in perch height in anoles are often ascribed to differing resource needs or
competition (e.g. Andrews 1971; Lister 1976; Rummel and Roughgarden 1985; Schoener
1975; Schoener and Gorman 1968; Scott et al. 1976). The differences found between
morphs in N. humilis were small, but consistent. If this is the result of competition
between females, it is not clear what the females are competing for.
Furthermore, the difference in predation on morphs based on the background they
are seen against (Chapter 5) combined with similar survival rates (Chapter 4) suggests the
morphs may trade-off between predation and another aspect of survival, such as
parasitism (Losey et al. 1997). Indeed, in another female polymorphic anole, density-
dependent differences in immuno-competence were found between morphs (Calsbeek et
al. 2008). Alternatively, morph frequencies may have reached equilibrium (Bond and
Kamil 1998). Moreover, females with multiple mating produce genetically diverse
offspring for which the combined genetic diversity approaches population variation
(Calsbeek et al. 2007). Hence, maintenance of female morphs may not require strong
selective pressures, and any selection acting on the morphs that contributes to their
maintenance may prove difficult to detect. Before continuing research on possible
110
selective pressures involved in maintaining female polymorphism in anoles, a worthwhile
endeavor would be to develop theoretical models incorporating anole biology that allow
exploring the relative role of neutral and selective processes.
In conclusion, several interesting patterns were revealed regarding the occurrence
of female polymorphism in anoles. The geographic distribution of this trait, along with its
association with habitat use deserves further investigation. Perch use differences between
morphs appeared consistent, even when compared among species, and this may hold a
clue to the processes involved in maintaining the morphs. The most striking pattern,
however, was the constancy of frequencies of morphs over four sample periods in nearly
thirty years. Because random processes are expected to lead to the extinction of at least
one morph (Futuyma 1998), especially considering the low frequencies of the gynomorph
patterns, selective pressures can be expected to re-establish the balance. Experimental
manipulation of morph frequencies is therefore recommended. Further research could
potentially have a wide-ranging impact on our ideas about the evolution of color pattern
polymorphism, because drab color patterns as seen in female anoles are generally
assumed to have evolved in response to predation. After testing the two predator-based
mechanisms that could maintain polymorphism, however, I found no support that the
three alternative morphs in females of N. humilis are maintained by predation. Until
morph frequencies are manipulated, however, we should consider the possibility that
predator effects may have remained undetectable.
111
LITERATURE CITED
Abbott, J., and E. I. Svensson. 2005. Phenotypic and genetic variation in emergence and
development time of a trimorphic damselfly. Journal of Evolutionary Biology
18:1464-1470.
Abbott, J. K., S. Bensch, T. P. Gosden, and E. I. Svensson. 2008. Patterns of
differentiation in a colour polymorphism and in neutral markers reveal rapid
genetic changes in natural damselfly populations. Molecular Ecology 17:1597-
1604.
Allen, J. A. 1988. Frequency-Dependent Selection by Predators. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
319:485-503.
Allen, J. A., and B. Clarke. 1968. Evidence for Apostatic Selection by Wild Passerines.
Nature 220:501-502.
Allen, J. A., and B. C. Clarke. 1984. Frequency-Dependent Selection - Homage to
Poulton,E.B. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 23:15-18.
Allen, J. A., and M. E. Weale. 2005. Anti-apostatic selection by wild birds on quasi-
natural morphs of the land snail Cepaea hortensis: a generalised linear mixed
models approach. Oikos 108:335-343.
Amundsen, T. 2000a. Female ornaments: genetically correlated or sexually selected?
Pp. 133-154 in Y. Espmark, T. Amundsen and G. Rosenqvist, eds. Animal
signals: signalling and signal design in animal communication. Tapir Academic
Press, Trondheim, Norway.
112
Amundsen, T. 2000b. Why are female birds ornamented? Trends in Ecology &
Evolution 15:149-155.
Amundsen, T., and E. Forsgren. 2001. Male mate choice selects for female coloration in
a fish. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 98:13155-13160.
Amundsen, T., E. Forsgren, and L. T. T. Hansen. 1997. On the function of female
ornaments: male bluethroats prefer colourful females. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 264:1579-1586.
Amundsen, T., and H. P?rn. 2006. Female coloration: a review of functional and
nonfunctional hypotheses. in G. E. Hill and K. J. McGraw, eds. Bird coloration: .
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Andersson, M., and Y. Iwasa. 1996. Sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution
11:53-58.
Andersson, M. B. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Andres, J. A., and A. Cordero. 1999. The inheritance of female colour morphs in the
damselfly Ceriagrion tenellum (Odonata, Coenagrionidae). Heredity 82:328-335.
Andres, J. A., R. A. Sanchez-Guillen, and A. C. Rivera. 2000. Molecular evidence for
selection on female color polymorphism in the damselfly Ischnura graellsii.
Evolution 54:2156-2161.
Andres, J. A., R. A. Sanchez-Guillen, and A. C. Rivera. 2002. Evolution of female
colour polymorphism in damselflies: testing the hypotheses. Animal Behaviour
63:677-685.
113
Andrews, R. M. 1971. Structural Habitat and Time Budget of a Tropical Anolis Lizard.
Ecology 52:262-&.
Andrews, R. M. 1976. Growth-Rate in Island and Mainland Anoline Lizards.
Copeia:477-482.
Andrews, R. M. 1979. Evolution of life histories: a comparison of Anolis lizards from
matched island and mainland habitats. Brevoria 454:1-51.
Andrews, R. M. 1985. Mate Choice by Females of the Lizard, Anolis-Carolinensis.
Journal of Herpetology 19:284-289.
Andrews, R. M. 1991. Population Stability of a Tropical Lizard. Ecology 72:1204-1217.
Avila-Pires, T. C. S. 1995. Lizards of Brazilian Amazonia (Reptilia, Squamata).
Zoologische Verhandelingen 299:1-706.
Ayala, F. J. 1972. Frequency-dependent mating advantage in Drosophila Behavrior
Genetics 2:85-91.
Ayala, F. J., and C. A. Campbell. 1974. Frequency-Dependent Selection. Annual
Review of Ecology and Systematics 5:115-138.
Barth, F. G., T. Nakagawa, and E. Eguchi. 1993. Vision in the ctenid spider cupiennius
salei: spectral range and absolute sensitivity. The Journal of Experimental
Biology 181:63-80.
Basolo, A. 2006. Genetic Linkage and Color Polymorphism in the Southern Platyfish
(Xiphophorus maculatus): A Model System for Studies of Color Pattern
Evolution. . Zebrafish 3:65-83.
Bateman, A. J. 1948. Intra-Sexual Selection in Drosophila-Melanogaster. Heredity
2:277-277.
114
Bateson, W. 1894. Materials for the study of variation. Mcmillan, London.
Bauwens, D., and C. Thoen. 1981. Escape Tactics and Vulnerability to Predation
Associated with Reproduction in the Lizard Lacerta-Vivipara. Journal of Animal
Ecology 50:733-743.
Bergsten, J., A. Toyra, and A. N. Nilsson. 2001. Intraspecific variation and intersexual
correlation in secondary sexual characters of three diving beetles (Coleoptera :
Dytiscidae). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 73:221-232.
Beuttell, K., and J. B. Losos. 1999. Ecological morphology of Caribbean anoles.
Herpetological Monographs:1-28.
Bittner, T. D. 2003. Polymorphic clay models of Thamnophis sirtalis suggest patterns of
avian predation. Ohio Journal of Science 103:62-66.
Bond, A. B. 1983. Visual-Search and Selection of Natural Stimuli in the Pigeon - the
Attention Threshold Hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal
Behavior Processes 9:292-306.
Bond, A. B. 2007. The evolution of color polymorphism: Crypticity searching images,
and apostatic selection. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics
38:489-514.
Bond, A. B., and A. C. Kamil. 1998. Apostatic selection by blue jays produces balanced
polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature 395:594-596.
Bond, A. B., and A. C. Kamil. 2002. Visual predators select for crypticity and
polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature 415:609-613.
115
Bond, A. B., and A. C. Kamil. 2006. Spatial heterogeneity, predator cognition, and the
evolution of color polymorphism in virtual prey. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103:3214-3219.
Bots, J., L. De Bruyn, S. Van Dongen, R. Smolders, and H. Van Gossum. 2009. Female
polymorphism, condition differences, and variation in male harassment and
ambient temperature. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 97:545-554.
Brodie, E. D. 1989. Genetic Correlations between Morphology and Antipredator
Behavior in Natural-Populations of the Garter Snake Thamnophis-Ordinoides.
Nature 342:542-543.
Brodie, E. D. 1992. Correlational Selection for Color Pattern and Antipredator Behavior
in the Garter Snake Thamnophis-Ordinoides. Evolution 46:1284-1298.
Brodie, E. D. 1993. Differential Avoidance of Coral Snake Banded Patterns by Free-
Ranging Avian Predators in Costa-Rica. Evolution 47:227-235.
Bromham, L. 2002. Molecular Clocks in Reptiles: Life History Influences Rate of
Molecular Evolution. Mol Biol Evol 19:302-309.
Brown, J. F. 1931a. The thresholds for visual movement. Psychological Research
14:249-268.
Brown, J. F. 1931b. The visual perception of velocity. Psychological Research 14:199-
232.
Burnell, K. L., and S. B. Hedges. 1990. Relationships of West Indian Anolis (Sauria:
Iguanidae): An approach using slow-evolving protein loci. Caribbean Journal of
Science 26:7-30.
116
Burnham, K. P., D. R. Anderson, G. C. White, C. Brownie, and P. H. Pollock. 1987.
Design and analysis methods for fish survival experiments based on release-
recapture. American Fisheries Society, Bethesda, Maryland, USA.
Buskirk, R. E. 1985. Zoogeographic Patterns and Tectonic History of Jamaica and the
Northern Caribbean. Journal of Biogeography 12:445-461.
Butler, M. A. 2007. Vive le difference! Sexual dimorphism and adaptive patterns in
lizards of the genus Anolis. Integr. Comp. Biol. 47:272-284.
Butler, M. A., and J. B. Losos. 2002. Multivariate sexual dimorphism, sexual selection,
and adaptation in Greater Antillean Anolis lizards. Ecological Monographs
72:541-559.
Cain, A. J., and P. M. Sheppard. 1954a. Natural Selection in Cepaea. Genetics 39:89-
116.
Cain, A. J., and P. M. Sheppard. 1954b. The Theory of Adaptive Polymorphism.
American Naturalist 88:321-326.
Calsbeek, R., C. Bonneaud, S. Prabhu, N. Manoukis, and T. B. Smith. 2007. Multiple
paternity and sperm storage lead to increased genetic diversity in Anolis lizards.
Evolutionary Ecology Research 9:495-503.
Calsbeek, R., C. Bonneaud, and T. B. Smith. 2008. Differential fitness effects of
immunocompetence and neighbourhood density in alternative female lizard
morphs. Journal of Animal Ecology 77:103-109.
Campbell, J. A. 1998 Amphibians and reptiles of Northern Guatemala, the Yucatan, and
Belize. University of Oklahoma Press, Norman, OK.
117
Cannatella, D. C., and K. de Queiroz. 1989. Phylogenetic Systematics of the Anoles - Is
a New Taxonomy Warranted. Systematic Zoology 38:57-69.
Carey, V. J. 2007. gee: Generalized Estimation Equation solver. R package version
4.13-13.
Cast, E. E., M. E. Gifford, K. R. Schneider, A. J. Hardwick, J. S. Parmerlee, and R.
Powell. 2000. Natural history of an anoline lizard community in the Sierra de
Baoruco, Dominican Republic. Caribbean Journal of Science 36:258-266.
Charlesworth, B. 1984. The evolutionary genetics of life histories. Pp. 117-133 in B.
Shorrocks, ed. Evolutionary Ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, U.K.
Charlesworth, B. 1987. The heritability of fitness. Pp. 21-40 in J. W. Bradbury and M.
B. Anderson, eds. Sexual selectin: testing the alternatives. Wiley, Chichester,
U.K.
Cheverud, J. M., M. M. Dow, and W. Leutenegger. 1985. The Quantitative Assessment
of Phylogenetic Constraints in Comparative Analyses - Sexual Dimorphism in
Body-Weight among Primates. Evolution 39:1335-1351.
Church, S. C., M. Jowers, and J. A. Allen. 1997. Does prey dispersion affect frequency-
dependent predation by wild birds? Oecologia 111:292-296.
Clark, W. C. 1976. The environment and the genotype in polymorphism. Zoological
Journal of the Linnean Society 58:255-262.
Clarke, B., and P. O'Donald. 1964. Frequency-Dependent Selection. Heredity 19:201-&.
Clarke, C. A., and P. M. Sheppard. 1962. Genetics of Mimetic Butterfly Papilio
Glaucus. Ecology 43:159-&.
118
Clarke, C. A., and P. M. Sheppard. 1963. Interactions between Major Genes and
Polygenes in Determination of Mimetic Patterns of Papilio Dardanus. Evolution
17:404-&.
Coddington, J. A. 1990. Bridges between Evolutionary Pattern and Process. Cladistics-
the International Journal of the Willi Hennig Society 6:379-386.
Collette, B. B. 1961. Correlations between ecology and morphology in anoline lizards
from Havana, Cuba and southern Florida. . Bulletin of the Museum of
Comparative Zoology. 125:137-162.
Cook, L. M. 1998. A two-stage model for Cepaea polymorphism. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
353:1577-1593.
Cook, L. M. 2003. The rise and fall of the Carbonaria form of the peppered moth.
Quarterly Review of Biology 78:399-417.
Cordero, A. 1990. The Inheritance of Female Polymorphism in the Damselfly Ischnura-
Graellsii (Rambur) (Odonata, Coenagrionidae). Heredity 64:341-346.
Cordero, A. 1992. Density-Dependent Mating Success and Color Polymorphism in
Females of the Damselfly Ischnura-Graellsii (Odonata, Coenagrionidae). Journal
of Animal Ecology 61:769-780.
Cordero, A., S. S. Carbone, and C. Utzeri. 1998. Mating opportunities and mating costs
are reduced in androchrome female damselflies, Ischnura elegans (Odonata).
Animal Behaviour 55:185-197.
Cornette, J. L. 1981. Deterministic Genetic Models in Varying Environments. Journal of
Mathematical Biology 12:173-186.
119
Cott, H. B. 1940. Adaptive coloration in animals. Methuen & Co, London, U.K.
Crane, J. 1975. Fiddler Crabs of the World. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Craze, P. G. 2009. The fate of balanced, phenotypic polymorphisms in fragmented
metapopulations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22:1556-1561.
Creer, D. A., K. de Queiroz, T. R. Jackman, J. B. Losos, and A. Larson. 2001.
Systematics of the Anolis roquet series of the southern Lesser Antilles. Journal of
Herpetology 35:428-441.
Crother, B. I., and C. Guyer. 1996. Caribbean historical biogeography: Was the
dispersal-vicariance debate eliminated by an extraterrestrial bolide? Herpetologica
52:440-465.
Cunningham, C. W. 1999. Some limitations of ancestral character-state reconstruction
when testing evolutionary hypotheses. Systematic Biology 48:665-674.
Cunningham, C. W., K. E. Omland, and T. H. Oakley. 1998. Reconstructing ancestral
character states: a critical reappraisal. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13:361-366.
Cuthill, I. C., T. S. Troscianko, A. Kibblewhite, O. King, and M. Stevens. 2007. Edge
enhancement in disruptive camouflage. Perception 36:1.
Darwin, C. 1859. On the origin of species by the means of natural selection. Murray,
London.
Darwin, C. 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. John Murray,
London.
Davies, N. B. 1991. Mating systems. Pp. 169-196 in J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies, eds.
Behavioural ecology - An evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific
Publications, Oxford.
120
Dempster, E. R. 1955. Maintenance of Genetic Heterogeneity. Cold Spring Harbor
Symposia on Quantitative Biology 20:25-32.
Detto, T., J. M. Hemmi, and P. R. Y. Backwell. 2008. Colouration and Colour Changes
of the Fiddler Crab, Uca capricornis: A Descriptive Study. PLoS
ONE 3:e1629.
Dixon, J. R., and P. Soini. 1986. The reptiles of the upper Amazon Basin, Iquitos region,
Peru. Milwaukee Public Museum, Milwaukee.
Dobson, A. P., S. V. Pacala, J. D. Roughgarden, E. R. Carper, and E. A. Harris. 1992.
The Parasites of Anolis Lizards in the Northern Lesser Antilles .1. Patterns of
Distribution and Abundance. Oecologia 91:110-117.
Dobson, F. S. 1985. The Use of Phylogeny in Behavior and Ecology. Evolution
39:1384-1388.
Dobzhansky, T., and O. Pavlovsky. 1957. An Experimental-Study of Interaction
between Genetic Drift and Natural-Selection. Evolution 11:311-319.
Dollo, L. 1893. Les lois de l'?volution. Bulletin de la Societ? Belge de Geologie de
Paleontologie et d?Hydrologie 7:164-166.
Dominey, W. J. 1980. Female mimicry in male bluegill sunfish[mdash]a genetic
polymorphism? Nature 284:546-548.
Duellman, W. E. 1978. The biology of an equatorial herptofauna in Amazonian
Ecuador. Miscellaneous Publications of the Museum of Natural History of the
University of Kansas 65:1-352.
121
Duellman, W. E. 2005. Cusco Amaz?nico: the lives of amphibians and reptiles in an
Amazonian rainforest. . Comstock Publishing Associates, Cornell University
Press., Ithaca.
Duellman, W. E., and L. Trueb. 1986. Biology of Amphibians. Johns Hopkins
University Press, Baltimore, Maryland.
Dukas, R., and A. C. Kamil. 2001. Limited attention: the constraint underlying search
image. Behavioral Ecology 12:192-199.
Eales, J., R. S. Thorpe, and A. Malhotra. 2008. Weak founder effect signal in a recent
introduction of Caribbean Anolis. Molecular Ecology 17:1416-1426.
Edmunds, M. 1974. Defence in animals. Longman, Harlow, UK.
Endler, J. A. 1978. A predator's view of animal color patterns. Evolutionary Biology
11:319-364.
Endler, J. A. 1980. Natural-Selection on Color Patterns in Poecilia-Reticulata. Evolution
34:76-91.
Endler, J. A. 1984. Progressive Background in Moths, and a Quantitative Measure of
Crypsis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 22:187-231.
Endler, J. A. 1986. Natural selection in the wild. Princeton University Press, Princeton,
New Jersey.
Endler, J. A. 1988. Frequency-Dependent Predation, Crypsis and Aposematic
Coloration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-
Biological Sciences 319:505-523.
Endler, J. A. 1990. On the Measurement and Classification of Color in Studies of
Animal Color Patterns. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 41:315-352.
122
Endler, J. A. 1993. The Color of Light in Forests and Its Implications. Ecological
Monographs 63:1-27.
Endler, J. A. 1995. Multiple-Trait Coevolution and Environmental Gradients in
Guppies. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 10:22-29.
Endler, J. A., and J. J. D. Greenwood. 1988. Frequency-Dependent Predation, Crypsis
and Aposematic Coloration [and Discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 319:505-523.
Endler, J. A., and P. W. Mielke. 2005. Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see
them. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 86:405-431.
Estrada, A. R., and S. B. Hedges. 1995. A new species of Anolis (Sauria:Iguanidae)
from Eastern Cuba. Caribbean Journal of Science 31:65-72.
Etheridge, R. E. 1960. The relationships of the anoles (Reptilia: Sauria: Iguanidae): an
interpretation based on skeletal morphology. . Pp. 236. University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor.
Felsenstein, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the Comparative Method. American Naturalist
125:1-15.
Ferrari, F. D., and R. Bottger. 1986. Sexual Dimorphism and a Sex-Limited
Polymorphism in the Copepod Paroithona-Pacifica Nishida, 1985 (Cyclopoida,
Oithonidae) from the Red-Sea. Proceedings of the Biological Society of
Washington 99:274-285.
Fisher, R. A. 1922. On the dominance ratio. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
Edinburg 42:321-341.
Fisher, R. A. 1930. The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
123
Fisher, R. A., and E. B. Ford. 1929. The variability of species in the Lepidoptera, with
reference to abundance and sex. . Transactions of the Royal Entomological
Society of London 76:367-384.
Fitch, H. S. 1970. Reproductive cycles in lizards and snakes. University of Kansas
Museum of Natural History, Miscellaneous Publications 52:1-247.
Fitch, H. S. 1973. A field study of Costa Rican lizards. University of Kansas Science
Bulletin 50:39-126.
Fitch, H. S. 1975. Sympatry and interrelationships in Costa Rican anoles. Occasional
papers of the museum of natural history of the university of Kansas 40:1-60.
Fitzpatrick, B., K. Shook, and R. Izally. 2009. Frequency-dependent selection by wild
birds promotes polymorphism in model salamanders. BMC Ecology 9:12.
Fleishman, L. J. 1991. Design features of the displays of anoline lizards. Pp. 33-48 in J.
B. Losos and G. C. Mayer, eds. Anolis Newsletter IV. . National Museum of
Natural History, Smithsonian Institution., Washington, DC.
Ford, E. B. 1940. Polymorphism and Taxonomy. Pp. 493-513 in J. Huxley, ed. The New
Systematics. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
Ford, E. B. 1945. Polymorphism. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical
Society 20:73-88.
Forsman, A. 1997. Thermal capacity of different colour morphs in the pygmy
grasshopper Tetrix subulata. Annales Zoologici Fennici 34:145-149.
Forsman, A., and S. Appelqvist. 1998. Visual predators impose correlational selection
on prey color pattern and behavior. Behavioral Ecology 9:409-413.
124
Forsman, A., and R. Shine. 1995. The Adaptive Significance of Color Pattern
Polymorphism in the Australian Scincid Lizard Lampropholis-Delicata.
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 55:273-291.
Fox, J. 2009. car: Companion to Applied Regression. R package version 1.2-14.
Fox, W. 1963. Special Tubules for Sperm Storage in Female Lizards. Nature 198:500-
&.
Freckleton, R. P., P. H. Harvey, and M. Pagel. 2002. Phylogenetic analysis and
comparative data: A test and review of evidence. American Naturalist 160:712-
726.
Futuyma, D. 1998. Evolutionary Biology. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland,
Massachusetts.
Galeotti, P., D. Rubolini, P. O. Dunn, and M. Fasola. 2003. Colour polymorphism in
birds: causes and functions. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16:635-646.
Galiano, M. E. 1981. Revision Related to the Genus Phiale Koch,C.L., 1846 (Araneae,
Salticidae) .3. Polymorphic Species of the Mimica Group. Journal of Arachnology
9:61-85.
Garciadorado, A. 1986. The Effect of Niche Preference on Polymorphism Protection in
a Heterogeneous Environment. Evolution 40:936-945.
Garrido, O. H., and S. B. Hedges. 2001. A new anole from the northern slope of the
Sierra Maestra in eastern Cuba (Squamata : Iguanidae). Journal of Herpetology
35:378-383.
Gibson, A. R., and J. B. Falls. 1979. Thermal Biology of the Common Garter Snake
Thamnophis-Sirtalis (L) .2. Effects of Melanism. Oecologia 43:99-109.
125
Gillespie, J. H. 1973. Polymorphism in Random Environments. Theoretical Population
Biology 4:193-195.
Gillespie, J. H., and C. H. Langley. 1974. General Model to Account for Enzyme
Variation in Natural Populations. Genetics 76:837-884.
Gorman, G. C., and L. Atkins. 1969. The zoogeography of Lesser Antillean Anolis
lizards - an analysis based upon chromosomes and lactic dehydrogenases. Bulletin
of the Museum of Comparative Zoology. 138:53-80.
Gorman, G. C., and Y. J. Kim. 1976. Anolis Lizards of Eastern Caribbean - a Case-
Study in Evolution .2. Genetic Relationships and Genetic-Variation of
Bimaculatus Group. Systematic Zoology 25:62-77.
Gorman, G. C., Y. J. Kim, and S. Y. Yang. 1978. Genetics of Colonization - Loss of
Variability among Introduced Populations of Anolis Lizards (Reptilia, Lacertilia,
Iguanidae). Journal of Herpetology 12:47-51.
Grafen, A. 1989. The Phylogenetic Regression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 326:119-157.
Grandcolas, P., and C. D'Haese. 2003. Testing adaptation with phylogeny: how to
account for phylogenetic pattern and selective value together. Zoologica Scripta
32:483-490.
Graur, D., and W. Martin. 2004. Reading the entrails of chickens: molecular timescales
of evolution and the illusion of precision. Trends in Genetics 20:80-86.
Gray, S. M., and J. S. McKinnon. 2007. Linking color polymorphism maintenance and
speciation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 22:71-79.
126
Gross, M. R. 1996. Alternative reproductive strategies and tactics: Diversity within
sexes. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 11:92-98.
Gross, M. R., and E. L. Charnov. 1980. Alternative Male Life Histories in Bluegill
Sunfish. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America-Biological Sciences 77:6937-6940.
Gulick, J. T. 1873. On diversity of evolution under one set of external conditions.
Journal of the Linnean Society of London. Zoology 11:496-505.
Guyer, C. 1986. Seasonal Patterns of Reproduction of Norops-Humilis (Sauria,
Iguanidae) in Costa-Rica. Revista De Biologia Tropical 34:247-251.
Guyer, C. 1988a. Food Supplementation in a Tropical Mainland Anole, Norops-Humilis
- Demographic Effects. Ecology 69:350-361.
Guyer, C. 1988b. Food Supplementation in a Tropical Mainland Anole, Norops
Humilis: Effects on Individuals. Ecology 69:362-369.
Guyer, C. 1994. Mate limitation in male Norops humilis. Pp. 145-158 in L. J. Vitt and E.
R. Pianka, eds. Lizard ecology: the third generation. Princeton University Press,
Princeton.
Guyer, C., and B. I. Crother. 1996. Additional comments on the origin of the West
Indian herpetofauna. Herpetologica 52:620-622.
Guyer, C., and M. A. Donnelly. 2004. Amphibians and Reptiles of La Selva, Costa
Rica, and the Caribbean Slope. University of California Press, Berkeley,
California.
Guyer, C., and J. M. Savage. 1986. Cladistic Relationships among Anoles (Sauria,
Iguanidae). Systematic Zoology 35:509-531.
127
Guyer, C., and J. M. Savage. 1992. Anole Systematics Revisited. Systematic Biology
41:89-110.
Haldane, J. B. S. 1932. The causes of evolution. Longmans, Gree & Co. Limited,
London, New York.
Haldane, J. B. S. 1955. On the Biochemistry of Heterosis, and the Stabilization of
Polymorphism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological
Sciences 144:217-220.
Haldane, J. B. S., and S. D. Jayakar. 1962. Polymorphism Due to Selection of Varying
Direction. Journal of Genetics 58:237-&.
Hardin, J. W., and J. M. Hilbe. 2003. Generalized estimating equations. Chapman &
Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, FL.
Harmon, L., J. Weir, C. Brock, R. Glor, W. Challenger, and G. Hunt. 2008. geiger:
Analysis of evolutionary diversification. R package version 1.2-14.
Harmon, L. J., J. J. Kolbe, J. M. Cheverud, and J. B. Losos. 2005. Convergence and the
multidimensional niche. Evolution 59:409-421.
Harris, H. 1966. Enzyme Polymorphisms in Man. Proceedings of the Royal Society
Series B-Biological Sciences 164:298-&.
Harvey, P. H., and M. Pagel. 1991. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology.
Oxford University Press, Oxford, England.
Hass, C. A., S. B. Hedges, and L. R. Maxson. 1993. Molecular Insights into the
Relationships and Biogeography of West-Indian Anoline Lizards. Biochemical
Systematics and Ecology 21:97-114.
128
Hedges, S. B. 1996. Vicariance and dispersal in Caribbean biogeography. Herpetologica
52:466-473.
Hedges, S. B. 2006. Paleogeography of the Antilles and origin of West Indian terrestrial
vertebrates. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden 93:231-244.
Hedges, S. B., and K. L. Burnell. 1990. The Jamaican radiation of Anolis (Sauria:
Iguanidae): An analysis of relationships and biogeography using sequential
electrophoresis. Caribbean Journal of Science 26:31-44.
Hedges, S. B., C. A. Hass, and L. R. Maxson. 1994. Towards a Biogeography of the
Caribbean - Reply. Cladistics-the International Journal of the Willi Hennig
Society 10:43-55.
Hedrick, P. W. 1986. Genetic-Polymorphism in Heterogeneous Environments - a
Decade Later. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 17:535-566.
Hedrick, P. W. 1990. Genotypic-Specific Habitat Selection - a New Model and Its
Application. Heredity 65:145-149.
Hedrick, P. W. 1999. Genetics of Populations. Jones and Bartlett Publishers, Sudbury,
MA.
Hedrick, P. W., M. E. Ginevan, and E. P. Ewing. 1976. Genetic-Polymorphism in
Heterogeneous Environments. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 7:1-
32.
Henderson, R. W., and B. I. Crother. 1989. Biogeographic patterns of predation in West
Indian colubrid snakes. in C. A. Woods, ed. Biogeography of the West Indies:
Past, Present, and Future. Sandhill Crane Press, Gainesville, FL.
129
Henderson, R. W., and R. A. Sajdak. 1996. Diets of West Indian racers (Colubridae:
Alsophis): Composition and biogeographic implications. in R. Powell and R. W.
Henderson, eds. Contributions to West Indian Herpetology: A Tribute to Albert
Schwartz. Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles, Ithaca, NY.
Hill, G. E. 2006. Female mate choice for ornamental coloration. Pp. 137-200 in G. E.
Hill and K. J. McGraw, eds. Bird Coloration. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Hillman, S. S., and G. C. Gorman. 1977. Water-Loss, Desiccation Tolerance, and
Survival under Desiccating Conditions in 11 Species of Caribbean Anolis -
Evolutionary and Ecological Implications. Oecologia 29:105-116.
Hinnekint, B. O. N., and H. J. Dumont. 1989. Multi-Annual Cycles in Populations of
Ischnura-Elegans-Elegans Induced by Crowding and Mediated by Sexual
Aggression (Odonata, Coenagrionidae). Entomologia Generalis 14:161-166.
Hoffman, E. A., and M. S. Blouin. 2000. A review of colour and pattern polymorphisms
in anurans. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 70:633-665.
Hoffman, E. A., F. W. Schueler, A. G. Jones, and M. S. Blouin. 2006. An analysis of
selection on a colour polymorphism in the northern leopard frog. Molecular
Ecology 15:2627-2641.
Houde, A. E. 1997. Sex, Color, and Mate Choice in Guppies. Princeton University
Press, Princeton, NJ.
Howe, A., G. L. Lovei, and G. Nachman. 2009. Dummy caterpillars as a simple method
to assess predation rates on invertebrates in a tropical agroecosystem.
Entomologia Experimentalis Et Applicata 131:325-329.
130
Hubbard, S. F., R. M. Cook, J. G. Glover, and J. J. D. Greenwood. 1982. Apostatic
Selection as an Optimal Foraging Strategy. Journal of Animal Ecology 51:625-
633.
Husak, J. F., J. M. Macedonia, S. F. Fox, and R. C. Sauceda. 2006. Predation cost of
conspicuous male coloration in collared lizards (Crotaphytus collaris): An
experimental test using clay-covered model lizards. Ethology 112:572-580.
Huxley, J. 1955. Morphism and Evolution. Heredity 9:1-52.
Irschick, D. J., L. J. Vitt, P. A. Zani, and J. B. Losos. 1997. A comparison of
evolutionary radiations in mainland and Caribbean Anolis lizards. Ecology
78:2191-2203.
Iturralde-Vinent, M. A. 2006. Meso-Cenozoic Caribbean paleogeography: Implications
for the historical biogeography of the region. International Geology Review
48:791-827.
Jackman, T. R., A. Larson, K. de Queiroz, and J. B. Losos. 1999. Phylogenetic
relationships and tempo of early diversification in Anolis lizards. Systematic
Biology 48:254-285.
Jackman, T. R., J. B. Losos, A. Larson, and K. De Queiroz. 1997. Phylogenetic studies
of convergent adaptive radiations in Caribbean Anolis lizards in T. J. Givnish and
K. J. Sytsma, eds. Molecular evolution and adaptive radiation. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
JCVI. The Reptile Database. J. Craig Venter Institute.
Jeffrey, C., and C. David. 1980. Sperm transfer and storage in the lizard, Anolis
carolinensis. Journal of Morphology 163:331-348.
131
Jennions, M. D., and M. Petrie. 2000. Why do females mate multiply? A review of the
genetic benefits. Biological Reviews 75:21-64.
Johnson, C. 1964. Inheritance of Female Dimorphism in Damselfly Ischnura Damula.
Genetics 49:513-&.
Johnson, C. 1966. Genetics of Female Dimorphism in Ischnura Demorsa. Heredity
21:453-&.
Jones, D. F. 1917. Dominance of linked factors as a means of accounting for heterosis.
Genetics 2:446-479.
Jones, J. S., B. H. Leith, and P. Rawlings. 1977. Polymorphism in Cepaea: A Problem
with Too Many Solutions? Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 8:109-
143.
Joron, M. 2005. Polymorphic mimicry, microhabitat use, and sex-specific behaviour.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18:547-556.
Joron, M., and J. L. B. Mallet. 1998. Diversity in mimicry: paradox or paradigm?
Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13:461-466.
Jose, J., W. J. Puma-Villanueva, F. J. Von Zuben, and J. A. F. Diniz. 2008. Phylogenetic
inheritance of genetic variability produced by neutral models of evolution.
Genetics and Molecular Research 7:1327-1343.
Jukema, J., and T. Piersma. 2006. Permanent female mimics in a lekking shorebird.
Biology Letters 2:161-164.
Keller, L., and H. K. Reeve. 1995. Why Do Females Mate with Multiple Males - the
Sexually Selected Sperm Hypothesis. Pp. 291-315. Advances in the Study of
Behavior, Vol 24.
132
Kettlewell, H. B. D. 1955. Recognition of Appropriate Backgrounds by the Pale and
Black Phases of Lepidoptera. Nature 175:943-944.
Kettlewell, H. B. D., and D. L. T. Conn. 1977. Further Background-Choice Experiments
on Cryptic Lepidoptera. Journal of Zoology 181:371-376.
Kimura, M. 1983. The neutral theory of molecular evolution. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge.
Kimura, M. 1991. Recent development of the neutral theory viewed from the Wrightian
tradition of theoretical population genetics. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America 88:5969-5973.
Knox, A. K., J. B. Losos, and C. J. Schneider. 2001. Adaptive radiation versus
intraspecific differentiation: morphological variation in Caribbean Anolis lizards.
Journal of Evolutionary Biology 14:904-909.
K?hler, G., S. Alt, C. Gr?nfelder, M. Dehling, and J. Sunyer. 2006. Morphological
variation in Central American leaf-litter anoles: Norops humilis, N. quaggulus and
N. uniformis. Salamandra 42:239-254.
K?hler, G., J. R. McCranie, K. E. Nicholson, and J. Kreutz. 2003. Geographic variation
in hemipenaial morphology in Norops humilis (Peters 1963), and the systematic
status of Norops quaggulus (Cope 1885) (Reptilia, Squamata, Polychrotidae).
Senckenbergiana biologica 82:213-222.
Kolbe, J. J., R. E. Glor, L. R. Schettino, A. C. Lara, A. Larson, and J. B. Losos. 2007.
Multiple sources, admixture, and genetic variation in introduced Anolis lizard
populations. Conservation Biology 21:1612-1625.
133
Kolbe, J. J., R. E. Glor, L. R. G. Schettino, A. C. Lara, A. Larson, and J. B. Losos. 2004.
Genetic variation increases during biological invasion by a Cuban lizard. Nature
431:177-181.
Kolbe, J. J., A. Larson, J. B. Losos, and K. de Queiroz. 2008. Admixture determines
genetic diversity and population differentiation in the biological invasion of a
lizard species. Biology Letters 4:434-437.
Kono, H., P. J. Reid, and A. C. Kamil. 1998. The effect of background cuing on prey
detection. Animal Behaviour 56:963-972.
Kottek, M., J. Grieser, C. Beck, B. Rudolf, and F. Rubel. 2006. World Map of the
K?ppen-Geiger climate classification updated. . Meteorologische Zeitschrift
15:259-263.
Krebs, R. A., and D. A. West. 1988. Female Mate Preference and the Evolution of
Female-Limited Batesian Mimicry. Evolution 42:1101-1104.
Kunte, K. 2009. The Diversity and Evolution of Batesian Mimicry in Papilio
Swallowtail Butterflies. Evolution 63:2707-2716.
Kvarnemo, C., and I. Ahnesjo. 1996. The dynamics of operational sex ratios and
competition for mates. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 11:404-408.
Lailvaux, S. P., G. J. Alexander, and M. J. Whiting. 2003. Sex-based differences and
similarities in locomotor performance, thermal preferences, and escape behaviour
in the lizard Platysaurus intermedius wilhelmi. Physiological and Biochemical
Zoology 76:511-521.
Lamotte, M. 1952. Le r?le des fluctuations fortuites dans la diversit? des populations
naturelles de Cepaea nemoralis (L.). Heredity 6:333-343.
134
Lancaster, L. T., A. G. McAdam, J. C. Wingfield, and B. R. Sinervo. 2007. Adaptive
social and maternal induction of antipredator dorsal patterns in a lizard with
alternative social strategies. Ecology Letters 10:798-808.
Land, M. F., and F. G. Barth. 1992. The quality of vision in the ctenid spider Cupiennius
salei. J Exp Biol 164:227-242.
Lande, R. 1980. Sexual Dimorphism, Sexual Selection, and Adaptation in Polygenic
Characters. Evolution 34:292-305.
Lande, R. 1987. Genetic correlations between the sexes in the evolution of sexual
dimorphism and mating preferences. Pp. 83-95 in J. W. Bradbury and M. B.
Andersson, eds. Sexual selection: testing the alternatives. Wiley, London.
Lank, D. B., C. M. Smith, O. Hanotte, T. Burke, and F. Cooke. 1995. Genetic
Polymorphism for Alternative Mating-Behavior in Lekking Male Ruff
Philomachus-Pugnax. Nature 378:59-62.
Larson, A., and J. B. Losos. 1996. Phylogenetic systematics of adaptation. Pp. 187-220
in M. R. Rose and G. V. Lauder, eds. Adaptation. Academic Press, San Diego,
CA.
Lazell, J. D. 1972. The Anoles (Sauria, Iguanidae) of the Lesser Antilles. Bulletin of the
Museum of Comparative Zoology 143:1-115.
Leal, M., and J. B. Losos. 2000. Behavior and ecology of the Cuban "chipojos bobos"
Chamaeleolis barbatus and C. porcus. Journal of Herpetology 34:318-322.
Leal, M., and J. A. Rodriguez-Robles. 1997. Antipredator responses of the Puerto Rican
giant anole, Anolis cuvieri (Squamata: Polychrotidae). Biotropica 29:372-375.
135
Lebreton, J. D., K. P. Burnham, J. Clobert, and D. R. Anderson. 1992. Modeling
Survival and Testing Biological Hypotheses Using Marked Animals - a Unified
Approach with Case-Studies. Ecological Monographs 62:67-118.
Lee, J. C. 1996. Amphibians and reptiles of the Yucatan Peninsula. Cornell University
Press, Ithaca and London.
Lee, J. C. 2000. A field guide to the amphibians and reptiles of the Maya world. Cornell
University Press, Ithaca and London.
Lee, S. J., M. S. Witter, I. C. Cuthill, and A. R. Goldsmith. 1996. Reduction in escape
performance as a cost of reproduction in gravid starlings, Sturnus vulgaris.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
263:619-623.
Lenart, L. A., R. Powell, J. S. Parmerlee, A. Lathrop, and D. D. Smith. 1997. Anoline
diversity in three differentially altered habitats in the Sierra de Baoruco,
Republica Domimicana, Hispaniola. Biotropica 29:117-123.
Levene, H. 1953. Genetic Equilibrium When More Than One Ecological Niche is
Available. The American Naturalist 87:331.
Levins, R., and R. MacArthur. 1966. Maintenance of Genetic Polymorphism in a
Spatially Heterogeneous Environment - Variations On a Theme by Howard
Levene. American Naturalist 100:585-&.
Lewontin, R. C. 1958. A General Method for Investigating the Equilibrium of Gene
Frequency in a Population. Genetics 43:420-434.
Lewontin, R. C., and J. L. Hubby. 1966. A Moleuclar Approach to Study of Genic
Heterozygosity in Natural Populations .2. Amount of Variation and Degree of
136
Heterozygosity in Natural Populations of Drosophila Pseudoobscura. Genetics
54:595-&.
Lister, B. C. 1976. Nature of Niche Expansion in West-Indian Anolis Lizards .1.
Ecological Consequences of Reduced Competition. Evolution 30:659-676.
Losey, J. E., A. R. Ives, J. Harmon, F. Ballantyne, and C. Brown. 1997. A
polymorphism maintained by opposite patterns of parasitism and predation.
Nature 388:269-272.
Losos, J. B. 1990. Ecomorphology, Performance Capability, and Scaling of West-Indian
Anolis Lizards - an Evolutionary Analysis. Ecological Monographs 60:369-388.
Losos, J. B. 1992a. A Critical Comparison of the Taxon-Cycle and Character-
Displacement Models for Size Evolution of Anolis Lizards in the Lesser Antilles.
Copeia:279-288.
Losos, J. B. 1992b. The Evolution of Convergent Structure in Caribbean Anolis
Communities. Syst Biol 41:403-420.
Losos, J. B. 1994. Integrative Approaches to Evolutionary Ecology - Anolis Lizards as
Model Systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 25:467-493.
Losos, J. B. 2009. Lizards in an evolutionary tree: Ecology and adaptive radiation of
Anoles. University of California Press
Losos, J. B., and K. DeQueiroz. 1997. Evolutionary consequences of ecological release
in Caribbean Anolis lizards. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 61:459-
483.
137
Losos, J. B., R. E. Glor, J. J. Kolbe, and K. Nicholson. 2006a. Adaptation, speciation,
and convergence: A hierarchical analysis of adaptive radiation in Caribbean
Anolis lizards. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden 93:24-33.
Losos, J. B., T. R. Jackman, A. Larson, K. de Queiroz, and L. Rodriguez-Schettino.
1998. Contingency and determinism in replicated adaptive radiations of island
lizards. Science 279:2115-2118.
Losos, J. B., T. W. Schoener, R. B. Langerhans, and D. A. Spiller. 2006b. Rapid
temporal reversal in predator-driven natural selection. Science 314:1111-1111.
Lynch, J. F., E. S. Morton, and M. E. Vandervoort. 1985. Habitat Segregation between
the Sexes of Wintering Hooded Warblers (Wilsonia-Citrina). Auk 102:714-721.
Macedonia, J. M. 2001. Habitat light, colour variation, and ultraviolet reflectance in the
Grand Cayman anole, Anolis conspersus. Biological Journal of the Linnean
Society 73:299-320.
Macedonia, J. M., A. C. Echternacht, and J. W. Walguarnery. 2003. Color variation,
habitat light, and background contrast in Anolis carolinensis along a geographical
transect in Florida. Journal of Herpetology 37:467-478.
Maddison, W. P., and D. R. Maddison. 2008. Mesquite: A modular system for
evolutionary analysis.
Malhotra, A., and R. S. Thorpe. 1991. Experimental detection of rapid evolutionary
response in natural lizard populations. Nature 353:347-348.
McDonald, J. H. 2009. Handbook of Biological Statistics. Sparky House Publishing,
Baltimore, Maryland.
138
McLaughlin, J. F., and J. Roughgarden. 1989. Avian Predation on Anolis Lizards in the
Northeastern Caribbean - an Inter-Island Contrast. Ecology 70:617-628.
Merilaita, S. 2006. Frequency-dependent predation and maintenance of prey
polymorphism. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19:2022-2030.
Merilaita, S., and V. Jormalainen. 1997. Evolution of sex differences in microhabitat
choice and colour polymorphism in Idotea baltica. Animal Behaviour 54:769-778.
Merilaita, S., and J. Lind. 2005. Background-matching and disruptive coloration, and
the evolution of cryptic coloration. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological
Sciences 272:665-670.
Merilaita, S., A. Lyytinen, and J. Mappes. 2001. Selection for cryptic coloration in a
visually heterogeneous habitat. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London
Series B-Biological Sciences 268:1925-1929.
Merilaita, S., J. Tuomi, and V. Jormalainen. 1999. Optimization of cryptic coloration in
heterogeneous habitats. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 67:151-161.
Miller, M. N., and O. M. Fincke. 1999. Cues for mate recognition and the effect of prior
experience on mate recognition in Enallagma damselflies. Journal of Insect
Behavior 12:801-814.
Moermond, T. C. 1979. Habitat Constraints on the Behavior, Morphology, and
Community Structure of Anolis Lizards. Ecology 60:152-164.
Morey, S. R. 1990. Microhabitat Selection and Predation in the Pacific Treefrog,
Pseudacris-Regilla. Journal of Herpetology 24:292-296.
Nevo, E. 1978. Genetic variation in natural populations: Patterns and theory. Theoretical
Population Biology 13:121-177.
139
Nicholson, K. E. 2002. Phylogenetic analysis and a test of the current infrageneric
classification of Norops (beta Anolis). Herpetological Monographs:93-120.
Nicholson, K. E., R. E. Glor, J. J. Kolbe, A. Larson, S. B. Hedges, and J. B. Losos. 2005.
Mainland colonization by island lizards. Journal of Biogeography 32:929-938.
Nicholson, K. E., R. Ibanez, C. A. Jaramillo, and K. R. Lips. 2001. Morphological
variation in the tropical anole, Anolis casildae (Squamata : Polychrotidae).
Revista De Biologia Tropical 49:709-714.
Noonan, B. P., and A. A. Comeault. 2009. The role of predator selection on
polymorphic aposematic poison frogs. Biology Letters 5:51-54.
Olendorf, R., F. H. Rodd, D. Punzalan, A. E. Houde, C. Hurt, D. N. Reznick, and K. A.
Hughes. 2006. Frequency-dependent survival in natural guppy populations.
Nature 441:633-636.
Ord, T. J., and E. P. Martins. 2006. Tracing the origins of signal diversity in anole
lizards: phylogenetic approaches to inferring the evolution of complex behaviour.
Animal Behaviour 71:1411-1429.
Owen, D. 1980. Camouflage and mimicry. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Owen, D. F. 1971. Tropical butterflies: The ecology and behaviour of butterflies in the
tropics with special reference to African species. . Clarendon Press, Oxford.
Oxford, G. S. 2005. Genetic drift within a protected polymorphism: Enigmatic variation
in color-morph frequencies in the candy-stripe spider, Enoplognatha ovata.
Evolution 59:2170-2184.
Oxford, G. S., and R. G. Gillespie. 1998. Evolution and ecology of spider coloration.
Annual Review of Entomology 43:619-643.
140
Pagel, M. 1999. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401:877-
884.
Paradis, E., J. Claude, and K. Strimmer. 2004. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and
evolution in R language. Bioinformatics 20:289-290.
Parmelee, J. R., and C. Guyer. 1995. Sexual differences in foraging behavior of an
anoline lizard, Norops humilis. Journal of Herpetology 29:619-621.
Patterson, G. B., and C. H. Daugherty. 1990. 4 New Species and One New Subspecies
of Skinks, Genus Leiolopisma (Reptilia, Lacertilia, Scincidae) from New-
Zealand. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 20:65-84.
Perry, G. 1996. The evolution of sexual dimorphism in the lizard Anolis polylepis
(Iguania): Evidence from intraspecific variation in foraging behavior and diet.
Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 74:1238-1245.
Pindell, J. L. 1985. Alleghenian Reconstruction and Subsequent Evolution of the Gulf of
Mexico, Bahamas, and Proto-Caribbean. Tectonics 4:1-39.
Pinto, G., D. L. Mahler, L. J. Harmon, and J. B. Losos. 2008. Testing the island effect in
adaptive radiation: rates and patterns of morphological diversification in
Caribbean and mainland Anolis lizards. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-
Biological Sciences 275:2749-2757.
Pocklington, R., and L. M. Dill. 1995. Predation on Females or Males - Who Pays for
Bright Male Traits. Animal Behaviour 49:1122-1124.
Poe, S. 1998. Skull characters and the cladistic relationships of the Hispaniolan dwarf
twig Anolis. Herpetological Monographs 12:192-236.
Poe, S. 2004. Phylogeny of anoles. Herpetological Monographs:37-89.
141
Poe, S., J. R. Goheen, and E. P. Hulebak. 2007. Convergent exaptation and adaptation in
solitary island lizards. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
274:2231-2237.
Poulton, E. B. 1884. Notes upon, or suggested by the coulours, markings and protective
attitudes of certain Lepidopterou larvae and pupae, and of a phytophagous
hymenopterous larva. Transactions of the Entomological Society of London.
1884:27-60.
Poulton, E. B. 1890. The Colour of Animals: Their Meaning and Use. Kegan Paul,
Trench, Trubner, London.
Pradhan, G. R., and C. P. Van Schaik. 2009. Why do females find ornaments attractive?
The coercion-avoidance hypothesis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society
96:372-382.
Pregill, G. K., and B. I. Crother. 1999. Ecological and Historical Biogeogrpahy of the
Caribbean. Pp. 335-356 in B. I. Crother, ed. Caribbean Amphibians and Reptiles.
Academic Press, San Diego, California.
Punzalan, D., F. H. Rodd, and K. A. Hughes. 2005. Perceptual processes and the
maintenance of polymorphism through frequency-dependent predation.
Evolutionary Ecology 19:303-320.
R Development Core Team, T. 2008. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical
Computing. . R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
Rand, A. S., and S. S. Humphrey. 1968. Interspecific competition in the tropical rain
forest: ecological distribution among lizards at Belem, Paraguay. Proceedings of
the United States National Museum 125:1-17.
142
Rand, A. S., and E. E. Williams. 1969. The anoles of La Palma: aspects of their
ecological relationships. Brevoria 327:1-19.
Rappole, J. H., and D. W. Warner. 1980. Ecological aspects of migrant bird behavior in
Veracruz, Mexico. . Pp. 353-393 in A. Keast and E. S. Morton, eds. Migrant birds
in the Neotropics: Ecology, behavior, distribution, and conservation. Smithsonian
Institution Press, Washington, D.C.
Reagan, D. P. 1996. Anoline lizards. Pp. 322-345 in D. P. Reagan and R. B. Waide, eds.
The Food Web of a Tropical Rain Forest. University of Chicago Press, Chicago,
IL.
Reid, D. G. 1987. Natural-Selection for Apostasy and Crypsis Acting on the Shell Color
Polymorphism of a Mangrove Snail, Littoraria-Filosa (Sowerby) (Gastropoda,
Littorinidae). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 30:1-24.
Reinhardt, K., E. Harney, R. Naylor, S. Gorb, and M. T. Siva-Jothy. 2007. Female-
limited polymorphism in the copulatory organ of a traumatically inseminating
insect. American Naturalist 170:931-935.
Rendel, J. M. 1953. Heterosis. American Naturalist 87:129-138.
Richards, O. W. 1961. An Introduction to the Study of Polymorphism in Insects.
Symposium of the Royal Entomology Society of London 1:1-10.
Rivera, A. C., and J. A. Andr?s. 2001. Estimating female morph frequencies and male
mate preferences of polychromatic damselflies: a cautionary note. Animal
Behaviour 61:F1-F6.
Rivero, J. A. 1998. Los anfibios y reptiles de Puerto Rico. Universidad de Puerto Rico,
San Juan, Puerto Rico.
143
Robertson, H. M. 1985. Female Dimorphism and Mating-Behavior in a Damselfly,
Ischnura-Ramburi - Females Mimicking Males. Animal Behaviour 33:805-809.
Robinson, E. 1994. Jamaica. Pp. 111-127 in S. K. Donovan and T. A. Jackson, eds.
Caribbean geology: an introduction. University of West Indies Publishers
Association., Kingston, Jamaica.
Rosen, D. E. 1975. Vicariance Model of Caribbean Biogeography. Systematic Zoology
24:431-464.
Roulin, A. 2004. The evolution, maintenance and adaptive function of genetic colour
polymorphism in birds. Biological Reviews 79:815-848.
Rummel, J. D., and J. Roughgarden. 1985. Effects of Reduced Perch-Height Separation
on Competition between Two Anolis Lizards. Ecology 66:430-444.
Ruxton, G. D., T. N. Sherratt, and R. C. Speed. 2004. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary
ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford University Press,
Oxford.
Sadlier, R. A., D. J. Colgan, and G. M. Shea. 1993. Taxonomy and Distribution of the
Scincid Lizard Saproscincus challengeri and Related species in Southeastern
Australia. Memoirs of the Queensland Museum 34:139-158.
Sandoval, C. P. 1994. Differential Visual Predation on Morphs of Timema-Cristinae
(Phasmatodeae, Timemidae) and Its Consequences for Host-Range. Biological
Journal of the Linnean Society 52:341-356.
Saporito, R. A., R. Zuercher, M. Roberts, K. G. Gerow, and M. A. Donnelly. 2007.
Experimental evidence for aposematism in the dendrobatid poison frog Oophaga
pumilio. Copeia:1006-1011.
144
Savage, J. M. 1966. An Extraordinary New Toad (Bufo) from Costa Rica. Revista De
Biologia Tropical 14:153-167.
Savage, J. M. 2002. The Amphibians and Reptiles of Costa Rica: A Herpetofauna
between Two Continents, between Two Seas. The University of Chicago Press,
Chicago.
Schall, J. J., and A. R. Pearson. 2000. Body condition of a Puerto Rican anole, Anolis
gundlachi: Effect of a malaria parasite and weather variation. Journal of
Herpetology 34:489-491.
Schluter, D. 2000. The ecology of adaptive radiations. Oxford University Press, Oxford,
England.
Schluter, D., T. Price, A. O. Mooers, and D. Ludwig. 1997. Likelihood of ancestor
states in adaptive radiation. Evolution 51:1699-1711.
Schmid, A. 1998. Different functions of different eye types in the spider Cupiennius
salei. J Exp Biol 201:221-225.
Schmidt, P. S., and D. M. Rand. 2001. Adaptive maintenance of genetic polymorphism
in an intertidal barnacle: Habitat- and life-stage-specific survivorship of Mpi
genotypes. Evolution 55:1336-1344.
Schoener, T. W. 1967. The Ecological Significance of Sexual Dimorphism in Size in the
Lizard Anolis conspersus. Science 155:474-477.
Schoener, T. W. 1975. Presence and Absence of Habitat Shift in Some Widespread
Lizard Species. Ecological Monographs 45:233-258.
Schoener, T. W. 1985. Are Lizard Population Sizes Unusually Constant through Time.
American Naturalist 126:633-641.
145
Schoener, T. W., and G. C. Gorman. 1968. Some Niche Differences in 3 Lesser
Antillean Lizards of Genus Anolis. Ecology 49:819-&.
Schoener, T. W., and A. Schoener. 1976. Ecological Context of Female Pattern
Polymorphism in Lizard Anolis-Sagrei. Evolution 30:650-658.
Schoener, T. W., and A. Schoener. 1982. The Ecological Correlates of Survival in Some
Bahamian Anolis Lizards. Oikos 39:1-16.
Schwartz, A., and R. W. Henderson. 1991. Amphibians and reptiles of the West Indies:
descriptions, distributions, and natural history. University of Florida Press,
Gainesville, FL.
Schwarzkopf, L., and R. Shine. 1992. Costs of Reproduction in Lizards - Escape Tactics
and Susceptibility to Predation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 31:17-25.
Scott, N. J. J., D. E. Wilson, C. Jones, and R. M. Andrews. 1976. The choice of perch
dimensions by lizards of the genus Anolis (Reptilia, Lacertilia, Iguanidae).
Journal of Herpetology 10:75-84.
Seehausen, O., P. J. Mayhew, and J. J. M. Van Alphen. 1999. Evolution of colour
patterns in East African cichlid fish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 12:514-534.
Sever, D., M. , and W. Hamlett, C. . 2002. Female sperm storage in reptiles. Journal of
Experimental Zoology 292:187-199.
Sheppard, P. M. 1967. Natural selection and heredity. Hutchinson., London.
Sherratt, T. N. 2001. The evolution of female-limited polymorphisms in damselflies: a
signal detection model. Ecology Letters 4:22-29.
Shine, R. 1979. Sexual Selection and Sexual Dimorphism in the Amphibia. Copeia:297-
306.
146
Shuster, S. M., and M. J. Wade. 2003. Mating systems and strategies. Princeton
University Press, Princeton.
Simpson, G. G. 1953. The major features of evolution. Columbia University Press, New
York.
Sinervo, A., and K. R. Zamudio. 2001. The evolution of alternative reproductive
strategies: Fitness differential, heritability, and genetic correlation between the
sexes. Journal of Heredity 92:198-205.
Sinervo, B., and C. M. Lively. 1996. The rock-paper-scissors game and the evolution of
alternative male strategies. Nature 380:240-243.
Sinervo, B., E. Svensson, and T. Comendant. 2000. Density cycles and an offspring
quantity and quality game driven by natural selection. Nature 406:985-988.
Sirot, L. K., H. J. Brockmann, C. Marinis, and G. Muschett. 2003. Maintenance of a
female-limited polymorphism in Ischnura ramburi (Zygoptera : Coenagrionidae).
Animal Behaviour 66:763-775.
Slatkin, M. 1984. Ecological Causes of Sexual Dimorphism. Evolution 38:622-630.
Smith, D. A. S. 1975. Genetics of Some Polymorphic Forms of the African Butterfly
Danaus chrysippus L. (Lepidoptera: Danaidae). Insect Systematics &
Evolution 6:134-144.
Smith, H. M. 1946. Handbook of Lizards. Comstock, New York.
Staddon, J. E. R., and R. P. Gendron. 1983. Optimal Detection of Cryptic Prey May
Lead to Predator Switching. American Naturalist 122:843-848.
Stafford, P. J., and J. R. Meyer. 2000. A guide to the reptiles of Belize. Academic Press,
San Diego/San Francisco/New York/Boston/London/Sydney/Tokyo.
147
Stamps, J. A. 1983. Sexual selection, sexual dimporphism, and territoriality. Pp. 169-
204 in R. B. Huey, E. R. Pianka and T. W. Schoener, eds. Lizard ecology: studies
of a model organism. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Stamps, J. A., and S. M. Gon. 1983. Sex-Biased Pattern Variation in the Prey of Birds.
Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 14:231-253.
Steffen, J. E. 2009. Perch-height specific predation on tropical lizard clay models:
implications for habitat selection in mainland neotropical lizards. Revista De
Biologia Tropical 57:859-864.
Steffen, J. E. 2010. Perch height differences among female Anolis polylepis exhibiting
dorsal pattern polymorphism. Reptiles and Amphibians: Conservation and Natural
History 17:89-94.
Stejneger, L. 1900. Descriptions of two new lizards of the genus Anolis from Cocos and
Malpelo islands. Bulletin of the Museum of Comparative Zoology. Harvard.
36:161-163.
Stenson, A. G., R. S. Thorpe, and A. Malhotra. 2004. Evolutionary differentiation of
bimaculatus group anoles based on analyses of mtDNA and microsatellite data.
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 32:1-10.
Stevens, M. 2007. Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of
protective coloration. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences
274:1457-1464.
Stevens, M., and I. C. Cuthill. 2006. Disruptive coloration, crypsis and edge detection in
early visual processing. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences
273:2141-2147.
148
Svensson, E. I., J. K. Abbott, T. P. Gosden, and A. Coreau. 2009. Female
polymorphisms, sexual conflict and limits to speciation processes in animals.
Evolutionary Ecology 23:93-108.
Talbot, J. J. 1979a. Time budget, niche overlap, inter- and intraspecific aggression in
Anolis humilis and Anolis limifrons in Costa Rica. . Copeia 1979:472-481.
Talbot, J. J. 1979b. Time Budget, Niche Overlap, Interspecific and Intraspecific
Aggression in Anolis-Humlis and a Anolis-Limifrons from Costa-Rica.
Copeia:472-481.
Thomas, G. H., S. Meiri, and A. B. Phillimore. 2009. Body Size Diversification in
Anolis: Novel Environment and Island Effects. Evolution 63:2017-2030.
Thorpe, R. S., A. G. Jones, A. Malhotra, and Y. Surget-Groba. 2008. Adaptive radiation
in Lesser Antillean lizards: molecular phylogenetics and species recognition in the
Lesser Antillean dwarf gecko complex, Sphaerodactylus fantasticus.
Molecular Ecology 17:1489-1504.
Tillyard, R. J. 1917. The Biology of Dragonflies. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Tinbergen, N. 1960. The natural control of insects in pine woods: Vol. I. Factors
influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. Archives Neelandaises de
Zoologie 13:265-343.
Tokarz, R. R. 1995. Mate choice in lizards: a review. Herpetological Monographs 9:17-
40.
Tokarz, R. R. 1998. Mating pattern in the lizard Anolis sagrei: Implications for mate
choice and sperm competition. Herpetologica 54:388-394.
149
Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pp. 378 in B. Campbell,
ed. Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871-1971. Aldine, Chicago.
van Buurt, G. 2005. Field guide to the amphibians and reptiles of Aruba, Cura?ao and
Bonaire. Edition Chimaira, Frankfurt am Main.
Van Gossum, H., and M. Y. Mattern. 2008. A phylogenetic perspective on absence and
presence of a sex-limited polymorphism. Animal Biology 58:257-273.
van Gossum, H., R. Stoks, and L. De Bruyn. 2001. Reversible frequency-dependent
switches in male mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series
B-Biological Sciences 268:83-85.
Van Gossum, H., R. Stoks, and L. De Bruyn. 2004. Conspicuous body coloration and
predation risk in damselflies : are andromorphs easier to detect than gynomorphs?
Belgian Journal of Zoology 134:37-40.
Vercken, E., and J. Clobert. 2008. Ventral colour polymorphism correlates with
alternative behavioural patterns in female common lizards (Lacerta vivipara).
Ecoscience 15:320-326.
Vercken, E., M. Massot, B. Sinervo, and J. Clobert. 2007. Colour variation and
alternative reproductive strategies in females of the common lizard Lacerta
vivipara. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:221-232.
Vitt, L. J., and S. de la Torre. 1996. A research guide to the lizards of Cuyabeno. Museo
de Zoologia (QCAZ) Centro de Biodiversidad y Ambiente Pontificia Universidad
Catolica del Ecuador
Voipio, P. 1951. The hepaticus variety and the juvenile plumage types of the Cuckoo.
Ornis Fennica 30:47.
150
Walla, P., F. G. Barth, and E. Eguchi. 2009. Spectral Sensitivity of Single Photoreceptor
Cells in the Eyes of the Ctenid Spider Cupiennius salei Keys. Zoological Science
13:199-202.
Wallace, B. 1975. Hard and Soft Selection Revisited. Evolution 29:465-473.
Watt, W. B. 1968. Adaptive Significance of Pigment Polymorphisms in Colias
Butterflies .I. Variation of Melanin Pigment in Relation to Thermoregulation.
Evolution 22:437-&.
Weiss, S. L. 2002. Reproductive signals of female lizards: Pattern of trait expression
and male response. Ethology 108:793-813.
West-Eberhard, M. J. 1983. Sexual Selection, Social Competition, and Speciation. The
Quarterly Review of Biology 58:155.
White, G. C., and K. P. Burnham. 1999. Program MARK: Survival estimation from
populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46:120-138.
Wickler, W. 1968. Mimicry in plants and animals (Translated by R.D. Martin).
McGraw-Hill, New York.
Wiens, J. J., M. C. Brandley, T. W. Reeder, and K. Schwenk. 2009. Why does a trait
evolve multiple times within a clade? Repeated evolution of snakelike body form
in squamate reptiles. Evolution 60:123-141.
Williams, E. E. 1969. Ecology of Colonization as Seen in Zoogeography of Anoline
Lizards on Small Islands. Quarterly Review of Biology 44:345-&.
Williams, E. E. 1972. The origin of faunas: a trial analysis. Evolutionary Biology 6:47-
89.
151
Williams, E. E. 1976. West Indian anoles: a taxonomic and evolutionary summary. 1.
Introduction and a species list. Brevoria 440:1-21.
Williams, E. E. 1983. Ecomorphs, faunas, island size, and diverse end points in island
radiations of Anolis. . Pp. 326-370 in R. B. Huey, E. R. Pianka and T. W.
Schoener, eds. Lizard ecology: studies of a model organism. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA.
Williams, E. E. 1989. A critique of Guyer and Savage (1986): cladistic relationships
among anoles (Sauria: Iguanidae): are the data available to reclassify the anoles?
Pp. 433-478 in C. A. Woods, ed. Biogeography of the West Indies. Sandhill
Crane Press, Gainesville, FL.
Wright, S. 1931. Evolution in Mendelian populations. Genetics 16:97-159.
Wright, S. 1948. On the roles of firected and random changes in gene frequency in the
genetics of populations. Evolution 2:279-294.
Wright, S. J. 1981. Extinction-Mediated Competition - the Anolis Lizards and
Insectivorous Birds of the West-Indies. American Naturalist 117:181-192.
Yan, J. 2002. Yet Another Package for Generalized Estimating Equations. R-News
2/3:12-14.
Yan, J., and J. P. Fine. 2004. Estimating Equations for Association Structures Statistics
in Medicine 23:859-880.
Yang, Z. 2006. Computational Molecular Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Zamudio, K. R., and E. Sinervo. 2000. Polygyny, mate-guarding, and posthumous
fertilization as alternative male mating strategies. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97:14427-14432.
152
Zhang, J. Z., and S. Kumar. 1997. Detection of convergent and parallel evolution at the
amino acid sequence level. Molecular Biology and Evolution 14:527-536.
Zuur, A. F., E. N. Ieno, N. J. Walker, A. A. Savelieve, and G. M. Smith. 2009. Mixed
effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer, New York.
153