The Influence of Family Routines on the Resilience of Low-Income Preschoolers 
by 
 
Larissa Katlin Ferretti 
 
 
 
 
A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty of 
Auburn University 
in partial fulfillment of the 
requirements for the Degree of 
Master of Science 
 
Auburn, Alabama 
August 6, 2011 
 
 
 
 
Keywords: family routines, preschoolers, resilience, prevention 
 
Copyright 2011 by Larissa Katlin Ferretti 
 
 
Approved by 
 
Kristen Bub, Chair, Assistant Professor of Human Development and Family Studies 
Ellen Abell, Associate Professor of Human Development and Family Studies 
Jacquelyn Mize, Professor of Human Development and Family Studies 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
ii 
 
Abstract 
 
 
Using data from the Birth to Three Phase (1996-2001) of the Early Head Start Research 
and Evaluation Project, I investigated whether family routines play a role in the development of 
young children?s social competence and cognitive ability above and beyond general parenting. In 
addition, the moderating effects of child gender and race/ethnicity were examined. Numerous 
findings were noteworthy. First, analyses revealed that routines do matter for child outcomes; 
whereas concurrent routines may be critical for fostering social competence at 36 months, early 
family routines may be more important for children?s 36-month cognitive skills. Second, when 
general parenting was taken into account family routines no longer predicted 36-month 
outcomes, due primarily to the high collinearity between these variables. Third, the effects of 
routines differed by child gender, with early routines having a stronger effect on girls? outcomes 
at 36 months and concurrent routines having a stronger effect on boys? outcomes. Associations  
also varied by race/ethnicity such that family routines moderately predicted child outcomes at 36 
months for European and African American children but not Hispanic children. Implications of 
these findings with respect to strength-based interventions for low-income preschoolers and their 
families are discussed.      
 
 
iii 
 
Acknowledgments 
 
 
 The author would like to thank her committee members, Dr. Jacquelyn Mize and Dr. 
Ellen Abel for their guidance and support throughout this Master?s thesis. She would also like to 
extend her sincerest gratitude to her major professor, Dr. Kristen Bub, for her continual 
encouragement, dedication, and guidance. A special thank you to her family, especially her 
parents, Rick and Lauren, her sister, Lia, her grandparents, Rich and Marie, and Brian, for their 
love and understanding, as well as the motivation they have provided her.  
 
 
                       
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
iv 
 
Table of Contents 
 
 
Abstract ......................................................................................................................................... ii 
Acknowledgments ....................................................................................................................... iii 
List of Tables ............................................................................................................................... vi 
List of Figures ............................................................................................................................. vii 
Introduction   ................................................................................................................................. 1 
Literature Review  ........................................................................................................................ 4 
 Poverty and Child Outcomes   .......................................................................................... 4 
 Parenting in Poverty   ........................................................................................................ 9 
 Family Routines   ............................................................................................................ 14 
 Resilience   ...................................................................................................................... 20 
 The Present Study  .......................................................................................................... 24 
Method  ....................................................................................................................................... 26 
 Sample ............................................................................................................................ 26 
 Procedure  ....................................................................................................................... 27 
 Measures  ........................................................................................................................ 28 
 Outcome Variables  ......................................................................................................... 28 
 Primary Predictors of Interest  ........................................................................................ 29 
 Control Variables  ........................................................................................................... 33
 
 
v 
 
Results  ........................................................................................................................................ 35 
 Analysis Plan  ................................................................................................................. 35 
 Preliminary Analyses  ..................................................................................................... 40 
 Research Question 1  ...................................................................................................... 45 
 Research Question 2  ...................................................................................................... 48 
  Research Question 3 ....................................................................................................... 49 
Discussion  .................................................................................................................................. 60 
 Limitations and Future Directions .................................................................................. 68 
 Implications..................................................................................................................... 71 
References  .................................................................................................................................. 72 
Appendices .................................................................................................................................. 89 
 Appendix A  .................................................................................................................... 90 
         Appendix B  .................................................................................................................... 91 
            Appendix C  .................................................................................................................. 120 
 
 
vi 
 
List of Tables 
 
 
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for All Study Variables  ............................................................. 91 
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics and Individual t-tests for All Study Variables by Gender  ......... 93 
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics and Individual ANOVAS for All Study Variables 
       by Race/Ethnicity  ................................................................................................................ 95 
Table 4. Correlations for All Study Variables for 14 months  .................................................... 97 
Table 5. Correlations for All Study Variables for 24 months  .................................................... 99 
Table 6. Correlations for All Study Variables for 36 months  .................................................. 101 
Table 7. Stability of Predictors Over Time  .............................................................................. 103 
Table 8. Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Routines 
       Measurement Model  ......................................................................................................... 104 
Table 9. Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Parenting 
       Measurement Model  ......................................................................................................... 105 
Table 10. Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Social 
       Outcomes Measurement Model  ........................................................................................ 106 
Table 11. Regression Model for Social Outcomes  .................................................................. 107 
Table 12. Regression Model for Cognitive Outcomes  ............................................................. 108 
Table 13. Regression Model for Observed Indicators of Routines at 36 months ..................... 109 
Table 14. Multi-Group Model for Gender and Social Outcomes  ............................................ 110 
Table 15. Multi-Group Model for Gender and Cognitive Outcomes ....................................... 112 
Table 16. Multi-Group Model for Race/Ethnicity and Social Outcomes ................................. 114 
Table 17. Multi-Group Model for Race/Ethnicity and Cognitive Outcomes ........................... 117
 
 
vii 
 
List of Figures 
 
 
Figure 1. Hypothesized Latent Growth Curve Models for Family Routines and Parenting. .... 120 
Figure 2. Hypothesized Regression Model  .............................................................................. 121 
Figure 3. Fitted Path Diagram for Social Outcomes Measurement Model  .............................. 122 
Figure 4. Fitted Path Diagram for Family Routines Measurement Model  .............................. 123 
Figure 5. Fitted Path Diagram for General Parenting Measurement Model  ............................ 124 
Figure 6. Multi-Group Model for Social Competence by Child Gender .................................. 125 
Figure 7. Multi-Group Model for Cognitive Ability by Child Gender ..................................... 126 
Figure 8. Multi-Group Model for Social Competence by Race/Ethnicity ................................ 127 
Figure 9. Multi-Group Model for Cognitive Ability by Race/Ethnicity ................................... 128 
  
 
 
viii 
 
Introduction 
In 2008, 19% of all children under age 18 in the United States lived in poverty. Young 
children are now the poorest age group in U.S. society (Economics and Statistics Administration, 
2009; National Research Council, 2000). A 2010 report from the Foundation for Child 
Development projects that by the end of 2010, 21% of U.S. children will be living below the 
poverty line, an increase of 4% in the last 4 years and the highest rate of child poverty in 20 
years (Land & The Foundation for Child Development, 2010). Past research has found strong 
associations between low-income and poor cognitive, social, and academic outcomes for 
children (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994; Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). These early 
deficits tend to have lasting effects, with low-income children being considerably more likely to 
drop out of school (Duncan, Ziol-Guest, & Kalil, 2010), commit a crime (Loeber & Farrington, 
2000), be unemployed and suffer from poor overall health as adolescents and adults (Duncan et 
al., 2010). Poverty among children birth to 6 can be especially devastating for children?s 
development since these years represent the period of greatest developmental vulnerability. 
Thus, identifying factors that support positive development for low-income children is critical. 
Resilience research suggests that sufficient positive assets can offset the detrimental 
effects of adversity or risk. More specifically, both individual and environmental protective 
factors buffer children from the effects of vulnerabilities, resulting in more positive outcomes 
(Masten, 2001; Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990; Patterson, 2002). Because parents and other 
caregivers are often the first ?environmental protective agents? that children experience, they are 
typically the most important and consistent protective factor for children (Hawley & DeHaan,
1 
 
 
2 
 
1996; Masten et al., 1990). Specific parenting practices, such as positive reinforcement, open 
displays of warmth and affection, and active monitoring of activities have been found to 
positively influence child development, especially in the early years (Kotchick & Forehand, 
2002). In addition, research focusing on school-aged children indicates that families who can 
create predictable routines despite challenging contexts may be able to protect their children 
from the stresses associated with inadequate economic resources (Brody & Flor, 1997; Fiese & 
Everhart, 2008). Routines around activities such as mealtime, play, reading, and bedtime create 
stability within the home, which allows for not only the completion of essential daily tasks, but 
also promotes the child?s social and cognitive development. Routines include family-level 
behaviors (e.g., mealtime) and child activities (e.g., homework time) that are supervised or 
arranged by an adult, all of which provide order, predictability, and structure to everyday life 
(McLoyd, Toyokawa, & Kaplan, 2008). Regular routines not only encourage family 
organization, but also provide a sense of belonging and increase family cohesion, which 
enhances child well-being (Fiese, 2002; Jensen, James, Boyce, & Hartnett, 1983; Systma, 
Kelley, & Wymer, 2001). Thus, routine practices may be especially important for families 
experiencing significant stress, challenges, or transitions (Fiese & Everhart, 2008; Fiese & 
Wamboldt, 2000). 
Despite emphasis frequently placed on the importance of family routines, we have little 
empirical knowledge about the role routines play in the development of very young children, 
specifically beyond the role of more general parenting. In addition, exploration of routines has 
generally focused only on either broad family routine use or on a single activity-specific routine 
such as bedtime or mealtime, and thus we know little about the role of activity-specific routines 
in children?s social and cognitive development or whether certain routines matter more for 
 
 
3 
 
certain outcomes. Finally, little is known about the buffering effect that routines can have on 
general parenting. Family routines may be particularly important for children and families in 
especially challenging environments because they may serve to reduce stress and chaos and thus 
protect against the adverse effects of poor parenting on preschoolers? social and cognitive 
outcomes.  
As such, the purpose of the current study was threefold. First, using a sample of low-
income preschool children and their families, I tested whether family routines are associated with 
more positive child social and cognitive outcomes above and beyond general parenting. I also 
examined activity-specific routines to allow for the exploration of differential effects of routines 
on child outcomes. Second, I explored the possibility that routines moderated the relationship 
between general parenting and child outcomes. That is, I examined whether children who are 
exposed to poor general parenting but who have regular bedtime, play, and reading routines 
exhibit better social and cognitive outcomes at 36 months than their peers with few routines. 
Finally, I investigated whether the effects of family routines on child outcomes differ by child 
gender and race/ethnicity. It is important to note that, unlike the majority of research on low-
income families, the current study focuses on the strengths of low-income families rather than on 
their deficits, a framework significantly underrepresented in the literature. The current study 
focuses on the presence of positive parenting (i.e., family routines, parental supportiveness, 
cognitive stimulation, and mild discipline) rather than the absence of harmful parenting (i.e., 
harsh parenting, neglect, or abuse). This approach will be more informative for creating 
prevention and intervention programs because it builds on the strengths that families already 
have rather than attempting to ?correct? what one deems they are not doing well.  
  
 
 
4 
 
Literature Review 
Poverty and Child Outcomes 
Economic inadequacies in childhood, as stated by Kaiser and Delaney (1996, p. 66), are 
rampant and the influences are ?multiple, diverse, direct, and indirect.? Poverty, defined by the 
U.S. government as three times the amount of income needed to buy a realistic amount of food 
for the family, is currently set at $22,050 for a family of four, and low-income, which refers to 
the condition of not having the means to afford basic human needs, have consistently been found 
to strongly predict poor social, cognitive, and academic outcomes for children. Poor children are 
more likely to have lower IQs, suffer from depression and peer-conflict, and be slower to gain 
language and literacy skills (Duncan et al., 1994; Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). Further, children 
from low-income families, despite being eager to learn, enter school with fewer academic skills 
than their more affluent peers, and considerable gaps in cognitive and academic competencies 
persist in later years (Stipek & Ryan, 1997).   
Social and behavioral outcomes. The link between family income and children?s social and 
behavioral outcomes has been strongly established in the literature (Linver, Brooks-Gunn, & 
Kohen, 2002; Mistry, Biesanz, Taylor, Burchinal, & Cox, 2004). Low-income children?s risk for 
parent-reported emotional and behavioral problems is 1.3 times higher than that for their more 
affluent peers (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Although poor social skills and problem 
behaviors are common in early childhood, and estimates suggest that approximately 10% of 
preschoolers exhibit noticeable social and behavioral problems, research by Webster-Stratton 
and Hammond (1998) indicates that base rates of aggressive and disruptive behavior are 
 
 
5 
 
significantly higher for low-income populations when compared to the general population of 
preschoolers (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, Skuban, & Horwitz, 2001; Qi & Kaiser, 2003). In a study 
of Head Start children, 23% to 33% presented aggressive behavior within the clinical range 
(Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1998). In a recent study of 3-year-olds and their mothers, 
Berger, Paxson, and Waldfogel (2009) reported that low-income children had more mother-
reported aggressive, withdrawn, and anxious behavior problems and more interviewer-reported 
behavior problems when compared to higher-income children. Likewise, Takeuchi, Williams, 
and Adair (1991) assert that parents? financial status and perceived financial stress have negative 
effects on children?s emotional and behavioral problems (e.g., impulsive, antisocial, and 
depressive behaviors). In their study of 7- to 11-year-old children, Takeuchi and his colleagues 
(1991) found that children whose families received welfare at two time points had more 
behavioral problems when compared to children whose families no longer received welfare at 
the second assessment.   
Cognitive outcomes. Although past research has found effects of income on children?s 
social and behavioral outcomes, effects of income are strongest on children?s ability and 
achievement-related outcomes (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Duncan, Yeung, Brooks-Gunn, 
& Smith, 1998). Low-income children are 1.4 times more likely to be diagnosed with learning 
disabilities or developmental delays when compared to their more affluent peers (Duncan & 
Brooks-Gunn, 2000). As noted by Smith, Brooks-Gunn, and Klebanov (1997) in their study of 
966 3- and 4-year old children, low-income children scored between 6 and 9 points lower on 
standardized tests of intelligence, verbal ability, and achievement. Although small, these are 
statistically significant effects. These differences were present even after controlling for maternal 
age, marital status, education, and ethnicity. Likewise, Duncan et al. (1994) reported that being 
 
 
6 
 
poor for the first four years of life is associated with a 9-point difference in IQ test scores at age 
5. Recent research indicates that socioeconomic disparities influence prefrontal (i.e., executive) 
functioning in children, such that children from low-income families have lower language, 
memory, and general cognitive processing abilities (Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry, & 
Knight, 2009).  
Persistent poverty. The detrimental effects of poverty on child outcomes are almost twice as 
large for children in persistent poverty when compared to those in transient poverty (Duncan et 
al., 1994). Persistent poverty is marked by continued environmental adversity over extended and 
consecutive years or assessment intervals (Ackerman, Brown, & Izard, 2004). Persistent poverty 
undermines the gradual development of cognitive and social competencies in childhood 
(Ackerman et al., 2004). Ackerman and his colleagues (2004) assert that repeated exposure to 
risk makes the exposure more stressful. Additionally, persistent poverty may hinder a child?s 
ability to recover from initial poverty deficits, in turn accentuating subsequent problems. In a 
study using data from children in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, Korenman, Miller, 
and Sjaastad (1995) explored the effects of persistent poverty from before birth, at birth, and 
throughout childhood (over a 13-year period). The researchers found that deficits in verbal, 
mathematical, and reading skills associated with persistent poverty were two or three times larger 
than those associated with the child?s current poverty status (Korenman et al., 1995). Likewise, 
in a study of low birthweight premature infants, Duncan and his colleagues (1994) found that the 
effects of persistent poverty were twice as large as the effects of transient poverty on 5-year-
olds? IQ scores. For four- to eight-year-olds, persistent low-income status was associated with 
internalizing problems (e.g., dependence, anxiety, unhappiness; McLeod & Shanahan, 1996) and 
externalizing problems (e.g., hyperactivity, defiance) even after controlling for poverty status, 
 
 
7 
 
mother?s age, education, and marital status (Duncan et al., 1994). More transient low-income 
situations were also associated with these problems, but not as strongly (Brooks-Gunn & 
Duncan, 1997).  
Timing. Research also indicates that income effects are largest for younger children as they 
are more reliant solely on their families during these years. In turn, they are more susceptible to 
the mediators of family income and child outcomes, such as a lack of home-based cognitive 
stimulation and unfavorable parenting practices (Linver et al., 2002). Moreover, studies of 
children?s early social and cognitive development indicate that family income in the first five 
years of life is highly correlated with later developmental outcomes (Duncan et al., 1998; Linver 
et al., 2002). Duncan and his colleagues (1997; 1998), in particular, found that a family?s income 
averaged from birth to five years of age had a more powerful effect on the amount of schooling a 
child completed than did family income measured between 5 and 10 or between 11 and 15. As 
Guo, Brooks-Gunn, and Harris (1996) assert, low-income is strongly related to low levels of 
preschool ability, which is associated with lower test scores in later childhood.  A recent study by 
Duncan et al. (2010) reported that poor children complete 2 fewer years of schooling, work 451 
fewer hours per year, earn less than half as much, and are more than twice as likely to report 
poor overall health or high levels of psychological distress as adults when compared to children 
whose families had incomes of at least twice the poverty line during their early childhood. 
Therefore, as the authors suggest, preschool development sets the stage for the transition into 
early childhood, adolescence, and even adulthood. 
Demographic differences in the effects of poverty. The extent to which, and the processes 
through which, poverty influences child development can vary by individual characteristics, such 
as gender and race/ethnicity. Research regarding the influence of gender on low-income 
 
 
8 
 
children?s outcomes is inconclusive. For example, some studies have demonstrated that boys 
suffer more adverse effects of poverty than do girls (Elder, 1979). C.J. Patterson and her 
colleagues (1990) found that boys from low-income families were less likely than other children 
(i.e., girls from low-income families, and boys and girls from more affluent families) to be 
competent across domains (i.e., conduct, peer relations, and achievement). In contrast, research 
by Petterson and Albers (2001) indicates that cognitive development scores were strongly related 
to income for preschool girls, but were only weakly related to income for preschool boys. These 
discrepancies indicate a clear need to further examine gender differences in the effects of poverty 
on child development.    
Research has also suggested that minority status may heighten the risk of developmental 
delays associated with low-income, in turn making it difficult to tease apart the effects of poverty 
from the effects of race/ethnicity (Patterson, Kupersmidt, & Vaden, 1990; Petterson & Albers, 
2001). Black and Hispanic children are more likely to live in poverty and to experience persistent 
poverty than are White children (Duncan et al., 1994). Thus, McLoyd (1990; 1998) suggests that 
Black and Hispanic children are at a higher risk than White children for experiencing 
developmental problems. Past research has demonstrated significant differences in behavior 
problems related to ethnicity (Gross, Sambrook, & Fogg, 1999; Leadbeater & Bishop, 1994). 
Minority mothers are more likely to report higher levels of problem behaviors and aggression 
than Caucasian mothers. In addition, Brooks-Gunn and her colleagues (1996) have found that 
low-income Black children do less well on cognitive tests than do low-income White children. 
Thus, examining racial/ethnic differences is critical for understanding the unique role that culture 
may play in supporting positive development among low-income children.      
 
 
 
9 
 
Parenting in Poverty 
The learning that takes place in a home before any formal education is instrumental in the 
future success of a child. The parent-child relationship inevitably plays a pivotal role in this 
learning (Halpern, 1990; Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). Specific parenting practices, such as positive 
reinforcement, open displays of warmth and affection, and active monitoring of activities have 
been found to positively influence child development (Kotchick & Forehand, 2002). More 
specifically, R. D. Conger and his colleagues (1992) found consistent, but not overly harsh, 
discipline practices were related to measures of adaptive child adjustment, such as self-
confidence, positive peer relations, and academic competence. In addition to teaching specific 
skills essential to early development, parents who use positive parenting practices can act as a 
buffer between their child and outside influences. Kaiser and Delaney suggest that parenting 
?becomes the medium through which the child experiences the world? (1996, p. 69). Thus, 
positive parenting strategies may be particularly important for children in stressful situations, 
such as poverty.   
Parenting practices are often influenced by the outside world against which parents are 
protecting their children. Poverty is one such influence. Kaiser and Delaney (1996) suggest that 
poverty harms family relationships by increasing conflict between spouses and children. 
Furthermore, poverty increases the likelihood that families will experience numerous risk factors 
simultaneously, thus exacerbating environmental stressors, leading to less optimal overall family 
functioning. Individuals parenting in low-income situations often deal with chronic stress, 
marital conflict, and depression (Halpern, 1990; Jackson, Brooks-Gunn, Huang, & Glassman, 
2000). In past research interviewing 35 high-risk low-income parents, parents reported that 
financial hardship was the primary barrier to parenting effectively. Parents frequently reported 
 
 
10 
 
being unable to meet the basic needs of their child, which led to their having negative feelings 
about themselves and their ability to parent (Russell, Harris, & Gockel, 2008). In addition, 
parents described a sense of marginality, or that their parenting ability was constantly being 
judged, which led them to question their own abilities and feel as though they should give up 
(Russell et al., 2008). 
Low-income parents more often use harsh, inconsistent, and physical discipline that is more 
likely to be coercive than do their affluent peers (Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). They tend to show 
less verbal and physical affection and are less likely to monitor their children successfully, both 
inside and outside the home, when compared to non-poor parents (Kaiser & Delaney, 1996; 
Kotchick & Forehand, 2002). Further, parent-child interactions among low-income families are 
frequently focused on obedience and conformity rather than positive reinforcement and open 
displays of affection (Kotchick & Forehand, 2002). The result is a more negative parent-child 
interaction.  
Economic strain is thought to indirectly influence preschoolers? outcomes through parent 
behavior (Guo & Harris, 2000; Jackson et al., 2000; Trentacosta et al., 2008). Consistent with the 
family stress model, which asserts children experience hardship not directly, but by the response 
of parents, Jackson and her colleagues (2000) reported that increased preschoolers? behavior 
problems were associated with lower levels of involved, supportive parenting among former 
welfare recipients (R. D. Conger & Conger, 2002). Furthermore, parents? discipline styles are 
significantly related to social competence in the classroom, such that harsher discipline, more 
often found among low-income parents, is associated with less prosocial child behavior 
(Anthony et al., 2005). In addition, Korenman et al. (1995) found that lower levels of emotional 
 
 
11 
 
support and cognitive stimulation provided by parents accounted for one third to one half of 
discrepancies seen in children?s reading, verbal, and math skills among the persistently poor.  
Guo and Harris (2000) explored this indirect relationship further and found that low-income 
was significantly related to lower levels of cognitive stimulation in the household (e.g., the 
number of books the child has, whether the child has a record/tape player, number of museum 
visits, how often the mother reads to the child, and number of magazines received by the family), 
unfavorable physical environments (e.g., home is not reasonably clean, home is cluttered, play 
environment appears unsafe, and home is dark), and adverse parenting styles. Further, these three 
mediating variables were found to have a significant effect on children?s intellectual 
development such that low-income children had lower scores on standard tests of cognitive 
development. The researchers noted that when the mediating variables were excluded from the 
analyses, poverty alone had no direct effect on children?s intellectual development. Thus, the 
effects of low-income were fully mediated through cognitive stimulation, the physical 
environment, and parenting style. Linver et al. (2002) also explored the role of family mediators 
in the association between income and child outcomes using a sample of preterm infants at 40 
weeks gestational age to 5 years of age. They found that parenting and the home environment 
mediated the relationship between income and child cognitive functioning and behaviors, even 
after controlling for child and maternal characteristics such as child birth weight, maternal 
marital status, education, and receptive verbal ability.  
The destructive influence of poverty on an individuals? ability to parent effectively is well 
supported by the literature (Kaiser & Delany, 1996; Kotchick & Forehand, 2002; Trentacosta et 
al., 2008); however, research has also found that many low-income parents are able to overcome 
or ?defy the odds.? A focus on the strengths of low-income families rather than their deficits, 
 
 
12 
 
especially for families with young children, is significantly underrepresented in the literature. 
The few studies that have been completed report that there are parents who are able to nurture 
and protect their children while utilizing effective parenting practices despite their poor financial 
situation (Brody, Flor, & Gibson, 1999; Koblinsky, Kuvalanka, & Randoplh, 2006; Maupin, 
Brophy-Herb, Schiffman, & Bocknek, 2010). In essence, as Halpern (1990) suggests, there are 
parents who are able to adapt or demonstrate parental resilience when facing high levels of risk.  
There is no clear consensus on what practices reflect positive parenting across various 
populations (i.e., ethnicities or income levels), however. Masten et al. (1990, p. 439) assert that 
effective parents are characterized ?by the degree to which their behaviors facilitate development 
for a particular child in a given cultural and historical context, not by some fixed set of ?good? 
parenting behaviors.? Past research has demonstrated that in the United States, a combination of 
warm and structured parenting is strongly tied to success in multiple domains and to resilience 
among children at risk (Kaminski, Perou, & Claussen, 2011; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998; 
Scaramella et al., 2011). However, the skills needed and practices used by low-income parents 
may differ from those of middle to high income parents due, in part, to the specific demands of 
the culture in which the child is growing up (Halpern, 1990). For example, Kotchick and 
Forehand (2002) suggest that the neighborhood in which a family lives affects parenting 
strategies. In extremely dangerous environments, effective parents often must be stricter, but still 
remain warm and caring. A focus on obedience and conformity may actually serve as a 
protective factor among families in unsafe neighborhoods where more restriction is needed in 
order to protect the child. Indeed parenting practices that emphasize parental control, monitoring, 
and supervision have been shown to help children adapt to and thrive in adverse environments 
(Halpern, 1990; Kotchick & Forehand, 2002).   
 
 
13 
 
In contrast to negative parenting practices, positive parenting practices, including maternal 
warmth, responsiveness, and consistency are associated with fewer internalizing and 
externalizing child behavior problems among low-income children (Koblinsky et al., 2006). The 
authors suggest that low-income mothers who demonstrate the aforementioned qualities model 
socially desirable behaviors and contribute to their children?s development of prosocial skills, 
including self-control and cooperative behavior. In addition, research by Brody and Flor (1997) 
exploring a sample of single African American mothers and their 6- to 9-year-old child found 
that mother-child relationship quality, specifically mother-child harmony during an observed 
interaction, was positively linked to child self-regulatory competence, which was linked 
positively with achievement and negatively with externalizing and internalizing problems. 
Moreover, mothers who believed they could influence their children?s development were more 
likely to encourage educational goals, respect for others in the community, and concern for 
others. In turn, these dyads were more likely to demonstrate mother-child harmony (Brody et al., 
1999).  
Parents vary on how they interpret and respond to financial and other stressors. Recent 
research indicates that parents? positive perception of resources and their ability to cope with 
stress are significantly related to higher quality parenting practices, regardless of actual formal 
governmental support (Maupin et al., 2010). Further, Jackson and her colleagues (2000) reported 
that children whose mothers reported high levels of emotional support and warmth, despite 
financial difficulty, had fewer problem behaviors and better preschool ability. In a study of 193 
African American single mothers and their 3- to 5-year old child, McGroder (2000) examined 
individual differences among low-income parents by identifying 4 specific parenting patterns. 
She found that Cognitively Stimulating mothers (i.e., read to child, talk to the child about his/her 
 
 
14 
 
day) and Patient/Nurturant mothers (i.e., warm and responsive) reported more favorable 
psychological well-being and their children displayed greater cognitive school readiness and 
social maturity compared to children of Low Nurturant and Aggravated/Nurturant mothers.  
Clearly, positive parenting practices can serve as a protective factor against the stresses 
associated with poverty. Yet, compared with studies that have examined the negative effects of 
poor parenting, relatively few studies have investigated the family processes that encourage 
prosocial behaviors and cognitive growth among young low-income children. If we are to 
develop effective programs and practices designed to support the development of children facing 
adversity, more research is needed to better understand which specific parenting practices and 
behaviors foster positive social and cognitive outcomes in very young children. One potential 
practice that may foster resilience but that has rarely been investigated, especially in very young 
children, is family routines.  
Family Routines  
Research indicates that unpredictability and chaos within a home are threats to children?s 
healthy development, in part because they are associated with consistently higher levels of stress 
(Koulouglioti, Cole, & Kitzman, 2009). Children in low-income families often live in homes that 
are overcrowded and noisy, and their home lives are more often hectic, unstructured, and 
unpredictable when compared to middle- and upper-income families (Evans, Gonnella, 
Marcynyszyn, Gentile, & Salpekar, 2005; Fiese, 2006). Evans and his colleagues (2001; 2005) 
assert that chaos severely damages a young child?s ability to self-regulate and thus manage his or 
her own behaviors and emotions, or attune to important information, processes that are critical 
for learning. Research suggests, however, that common daily routines around activities such as 
play, reading, mealtime, and bedtime create stability within a home. This stability allows for not 
 
 
15 
 
only the completion of essential daily tasks, but also promotes a child?s social and cognitive 
development (Fiese, 2006; Flores, 2004; Koblinsky et al., 2006). Routines have been defined as 
?observable, repetitive behaviors that involve two or more family members and occur with 
predictable regularity in the day-to-day and week-to-week life of the family? (Boyce, Jensen, 
James, & Peacock, 1983; Koblinksy et al., 2006, p. 555). Regular routines encourage family 
organization, but also provide a sense of belonging and increase family cohesion, which 
enhances child well-being (Fiese, 2002; Jensen et al., 1983; Systma et al., 2001). Healthy 
routines provide structure, but are also flexible and meet the needs of a changing family (Fiese & 
Wamboldt, 2000). Routines create stability, allow for a clear definition of roles and 
responsibilities within a family, and encourage parent-child relationships (Woods & Goldstein, 
2003). As humans, we search for order, particularly during periods of uncertainty (Boyce et al., 
1983). Thus, routine practices are important for families experiencing significant stress, 
challenges, or transitions (Fiese & Everhart, 2008; Fiese & Wamboldt, 2000).  
According to Boyce et al., routines are the ?organizational units of ordinary life in families? 
(1983, p. 194). In one of the first studies to investigate routines, Boyce and his colleagues (1983) 
observed 15 families 24 hours a day for a 3- to 7-day period and found that in all of the families 
observed, some degree of routine use was present. Some families had a highly-structured pattern 
of predictable routines for most activities, while others followed routines for only a few 
activities. Some routines were common, such as having dinner together each night, and others 
were very particular to a specific family, such as saying goodnight to the moon. The number, 
type, and importance of routines were not dependent on race or social class.  
It is thought that routines can buffer the effects of stressors and negative life events on 
children of all socioeconomic statuses (Boyce et al., 1983; Churchill & Stoneman, 2004). Recent 
 
 
16 
 
research indicates that family routines predict better physical health and academic performance, 
fewer school absences, better peer relations, and more positive parent-child relations among 
elementary school children two years after parental divorce (Guidubaldi, Cleminshaw, Perry, 
Nastasi, & Lightel, 1986). There is also evidence that regular routines, specifically family 
mealtimes, protect children of chronic alcoholics from developing problematic drinking habits 
(Bennett, Wolin, & Reiss, 1988). In addition, routines have been found to decrease disruptive 
behavior in children with disabilities (Lucyshyn, Albin, & Nixon, 1997), to facilitate good 
nutrition in infants diagnosed with failure-to-thrive (Yoos, Kitzman, & Cole, 1999), and to 
increase treatment adherence in families of children with asthma while decreasing child anxiety 
(Fiese & Wamboldt, 2000; Markson & Fiese, 2000).  
More recently, research has begun to explore the benefits of routines unique to low-income 
families. Family routines within low-income families have been linked to higher levels of self-
reliance, social competence, and academic achievement among adolescent and school-age 
children (Brody & Flor, 1997; Taylor, 1996). For example, in a study of 171 low-income African 
American mothers and their adolescents, Seaton and Taylor (2003) found routines were 
positively associated with academic self-concept and school engagement. Additionally, research 
by Loukas and Prelow (2004) indicates that family routines buffer the effects of cumulative risk 
(i.e., single-parent family, maternal distress, perceived financial strain, and neighborhood 
problems) on internalizing and externalizing problems in Latino adolescent females. Specifically 
Latino adolescent females in homes with more family routines scored lower on mother-reported 
adjustment measures when compared to peers without consistent family routines. Thus, the 
development of a stable and predictable home environment may be especially important for low-
 
 
17 
 
income children who are dealing with frequent stressors associated with a lack of economic 
resources (Seaton & Taylor, 2003). 
Despite emphasis frequently placed on the importance of family routines, we have little 
empirical knowledge about the role routines play in the development of very young children. The 
few studies that have examined routines in young children report positive effects similar to those 
for older children. More specifically, for low-income preschool children regular family routines 
are associated with more adequate sleep, fewer injuries, and increased ability to represent 
temporal relationships (Flores, 2004; Koulouglioti et al., 2009). Keltner (1990) explored family 
organization among 91 African-American Head Start families and found that children 
demonstrated more interest and participation in their environment when their families engaged in 
regular and predictable family interactions. In addition, routines were associated with more 
cooperative, compliant child behavior (Keltner, 1990). In a more recent study of 125 Head Start 
families, child externalizing behavior was negatively related to mothers? reports of how often 
routines were performed in their family (Churchill & Stoneman, 2004). As noted by Fiese 
(2002), the influences of routines during early development are not only immediate, but are 
evident years later. She found that family routines at four years of age predicted academic 
achievement at nine years of age. In addition, Fiese (2002) reported that dinnertime routines 
initiated in the preschool years remained relatively stable into the early school years, even for 
low-income families, and increased routine stability was related to increased academic 
achievement (Fiese, 2002). Finally, in a study examining positive parenting, family routines, 
family conflict, and maternal depression, Koblinsky et al. (2006) reported that children of 
mothers who participated in more family routines had greater social skills and exhibited more 
self-control and cooperation, as reported by the mother. The researchers found positive parenting 
 
 
18 
 
and family routines to be more salient in predicting children?s prosocial skills when compared to 
risk factors (i.e., maternal depression and family conflict) and thus concluded that positive 
features of low-income children?s experiences should be the emphasis of future research. 
Although difficult to disentangle from general positive parenting, there is evidence that routines 
matter in the development of young children.  
Demographic differences. Only a few investigators have examined possible child gender 
differences regarding the effects of routines on individual development and the findings have 
been inconsistent. In a study of the influence of routines on children?s post-divorce adjustment 
among school-age children, Guidubaldi and his colleagues (1986) found that regular bedtime 
routines predicted higher reading and spelling test scores, a better final grade in math, and higher 
teacher ratings of performance in reading and math for boys, but not for girls. On the other hand, 
regular bedtime predicted psychologists? and parents? ratings of the child?s happiness, number of 
close friends, and child report of perceived positivity in the relationship with the parent for girls, 
but not boys (Guidubaldi et al., 1986). In addition, among Latino adolescents, Loukas and 
Prelow (2004) found that irrespective of risk, girls exposed to more consistent family routines 
showed fewer internalizing and externalizing problems. These effects were not reported for boys. 
In a younger Head Start sample, Churchill and Stoneman (2004) found routines were more 
important when explaining girls? outcomes (i.e., conduct disorder, social competence with peers, 
and math ability) than when explaining boys? outcomes. The authors suggest that girls may be 
more sensitive to the home environment.  
 Likewise, a clear consensus has not been drawn regarding the influence of race/ethnicity on 
the association between family routines and child outcomes. (Fiese & Kline, 1993; Flores, 
Tomany-Korman, & Olson, 2005; Sytsma et al., 2001). Cultural variations in mealtime routine 
 
 
19 
 
practices have been widely studied, but we lack understanding of the differences in general 
routine use and more importantly in the benefits of routines for children in differing cultures. 
Despite the lack of focus on ethnic differences, experts in the field assert that family routines are 
an important ?purveyor? of culture (Fiese et al., 2002). Exploring cultural variations in routines 
provides a glimpse into what is relevant within a given ethnic group (Fiese, 2006). Fiese (2006) 
suggests that the activities of daily life reveal cultural values because families make decisions 
about how to organize their daily lives within the confines of cultural expectations. Culture 
influences child-rearing beliefs, the definition and role of family members, and the meaning of 
behavior, all of which are reflected in daily routines (Kubicek, 2002). Thus, there is evidence to 
suggest there may be differences in routine use and the influence of routines on child 
development. Exploratory studies of the source and impact of particular routines within different 
ethnic groups is needed.  
Clearly routines can create a secure, predictable, and organized home environment that may 
have particular benefits for low-income children. Still unanswered, but of theoretical and 
practical significance, however, is whether family routines are related to child outcomes after 
taking into account conventional measures of parenting (e.g., maternal warmth, involvement). 
Moreover, little is known about which specific family routines are most beneficial to very young 
children. Exploration of routines has generally focused only on broad family routine use or 
single-activity-specific routines (e.g., bedtime or mealtime) and thus we know little about 
whether certain activity-specific routines contribute more to positive development than other 
routines, as well as whether there are differential contributions of specific routines to specific 
outcomes for young children and families in challenging environments. The findings for school-
age children and adolescents are promising but not sufficient to generate policy or improve 
 
 
20 
 
practice for toddlers or preschool children. We also know little about the buffering effect that 
routines can have on general parenting. Exploring the possible moderating effects of family 
routines on the relationship between general parenting and child outcomes is crucial to the 
development of interventions utilizing routines. Family routines may reduce the connection 
between adverse parenting and preschoolers? poor cognitive and social outcomes. Routines can 
be a powerful family aid because relative to more general parenting practices or style (e.g., 
parental warmth), practitioners can teach families to establish home routines in a short period of 
time and without special resources (Koblinsky et al., 2006; Koulouglioti et al., 2009). 
Furthermore, routines offer an intervention tool that can be tailored to capitalize on a family?s 
strengths rather than their deficits (Fiese & Wamboldt, 2000; Koblinsky et al., 2006). Thus, it is 
critical, particularly with regards to efforts to increase school readiness among low-income 
preschoolers, to conduct additional studies examining the role that family routines play in the 
development of very young children. 
Resilience 
Children negotiate the stress of poverty in many different ways and with many different 
outcomes. Some children function at levels above that which is expected based on their risk 
experiences, while others suffer multiple setbacks and even maladjustment (Radke-Yarrow & 
Brown, 1993). Unfortunately, much less is known about children who successfully navigate 
poverty than about the problems associated with poverty. As Seccombe (2002) points out, most 
research has focused on problems instead of the ways vulnerable individuals avoid such 
problems. Resilience researchers strive to understand what characterizes adaptive individuals or 
families who are exposed to risks, be it individual characteristics or environmental factors 
(Garmezy, 1991). It is imperative that we learn how we can foster resilience in all children, 
 
 
21 
 
particularly at a time when so many of today?s children are facing adversity so that we can create 
more effective prevention/intervention programs.  
The term resilience is derived from Latin roots meaning ?to jump (or bounce) back? 
(Sillman, 1994 as cited by Seccombe, 2002). Resilience has been defined as a ?dynamic process 
encompassing positive adaptation within the context of significant adversity? (Luthar, Cicchetti, 
& Becker, 2000, p. 543). This definition focuses on resilience as a process that can change over 
time, not a static trait or disposition (Benzies & Mychasiuk, 2009). Although certain factors (e.g., 
behavior, emotion) may seem like characteristics of the child, they are often shaped by 
interactions between the child and aspects of his or her environment (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; 
Luthar et al., 2000). Resilience is a common phenomenon that occurs within normal 
development and results from basic human adaptational systems (Garmezy, 1991; Masten & 
Coatsworth, 1998). Thus, resilience does not imply immunity or invulnerability. Instead, resilient 
children and their families are able to use the resources around them as protective systems 
(Masten, 2001; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). The current study focuses on environmental 
factors, specifically parenting practices and family routines that foster resilience. 
Resilience is inhibited by risk factors and fostered by protective factors. Adversity or risk 
includes negative life circumstances that are known to be associated with adjustment difficulties 
(Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000). In other words, as Patterson (2002) notes, a risk is labeled significant 
when most individuals who are exposed to it show dysfunctional behavior. Masten and 
Coatsworth (1998) describe significant risk as: (a) high-risk status by virtue of continuous, 
chronic exposure to adverse social conditions (e.g., poverty); (b) exposure to a traumatic event or 
severe adversity (e.g., death of a parent); or (c) a combination of a high-risk status and a 
traumatic event. When demands placed on families exceed their capabilities, and there is an 
 
 
22 
 
imbalance, families experience crisis or a period of severe disorganization (Patterson, 2002). 
Risk factors increase the likelihood of poor outcomes among children (Benzies & Mychasiuk, 
2009). Distal risk factors, which include social class, are not directly experienced by the child but 
rather are mediated by proximal variables. Proximal risk factors (e.g., inadequate nutrition, 
marital discord, or parenting behavior) are experienced directly by the child. When distal risks 
are high, proximal factors become more important than when distal risk factors are low (Masten 
et al., 1990). Moreover, risks often ?cascade,? with one risk leading to another (Patterson, 2002). 
Protective factors, on the other hand, modify responses to adverse events so that families can 
overcome adversity. Family protective factors are those that shape the family?s ability to endure 
despite the presence of risk factors. Global factors associated with resilience include connections 
to competent and caring adults, cognitive and self-regulation skills, positive views of self, and 
motivation to be effective in the environment (Masten, 2001). Protective factors can emerge 
from individual family members (e.g., cognitive skills, self-regulation on the part of the child or 
parent), the family unit (e.g., parental warmth, family cohesion), and various community 
contexts (e.g., church involvement, good school system) (Garmezy, 1991; Luthar et al., 2000; 
Patterson, 2002; Seccombe, 2002). Each system of factors is unique yet interdependent (Hawley 
& DeHaan, 1996). Oftentimes, these supportive factors reinforce each other, thus enhancing the 
child?s prospects of sustaining positive functioning (Patterson, 2002; Radke-Yarrow & Brown, 
1993).  
Sufficient positive assets can offset the detrimental effects of adversity or risk, such that 
protective factors moderate the effects of vulnerabilities, resulting in more positive outcomes 
(Masten, 2001; Masten et al., 1990; Patterson, 2002). With higher levels of a buffer or buffers, 
adversity is less likely to have a negative impact on positive adaptation (R. D. Conger & Conger, 
 
 
23 
 
2002). In other words, protective factors serve as buffers and can actually change the nature of 
the relationship between adversity and positive adaptation. Positive adaptation is often defined as 
competence or success at stage-salient developmental tasks (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). 
Developmental tasks can be universal phenomena (e.g., language development) or can reflect 
more culturally or historically relevant (e.g., school achievement) stage-salient goals. From 
infancy to preschool examples of universal developmental tasks include attachment to 
caregivers, language development, and self-control and compliance (Masten, 2001). Luthar and 
Cicchetti (2000) note that positive adaptation may not refer to the presence of a positive trait 
(e.g., high cognitive ability), but rather to the absence of a negative trait (e.g., behavioral 
maladjustment). In addition, researchers suggest that a child does not have to be competent in all 
domains to be considered resilient (Luthar et al., 2000). Thus, it is possible that a child may show 
competence in one area, but not in others. 
An understanding of competence is dependent on the context in which the individual lives, 
and thus can sometimes be difficult to measure for cultures that are markedly different from the 
larger society (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). Because parents and other caregivers are often the 
first ?environmental protective agents,? they are typically the most important and consistent 
protective factor in children?s lives (Hawley & DeHaan, 1996; Masten et al., 1990). Masten and 
her colleagues (1990) assert that parents are able to enhance their child?s individual protective 
factors while also providing access to other family and community resources. Family cohesion 
and supportive parent-child interactions have been identified as some of the most significant 
protective factors for low-income families (R. D. Conger & Conger, 2002, 1992). Indeed, in low-
income families with high levels of warmth and cohesion, children perform better in school and 
are more likely to attend college (Orthner, Jones-Sanpei, & Williamson, 2004). Warm supportive 
 
 
24 
 
parenting behavior has also been found to mediate the association between economic inadequacy 
(a distal risk factor) and poor child outcomes (Mistry, Vandewater, Huston, & McLoyd, 2002; 
Yeung, Linver, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002). Further, parents who participate in frequent joint 
activities with their preschool children have been shown to protect their children from 
externalizing behavior and promote cognitive development. 
Masten and Coatsworth (1998) assert that studying children who overcome adversity to 
achieve good outcomes can teach us better ways to reduce risk and promote competence and thus 
is a critical step in generating effective policies and practices that support optimal development 
for families and children (Patterson, 2002). Past research has demonstrated that it is wiser to 
promote resilience as early as possible rather than try to implement treatments to repair existing 
problems among high-risk individuals (Benzies & Mychasiuk, 2009; Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000). 
However, little is known about which specific proximal factors may serve as protective factors 
among low-income preschool children. Identifying these key protective factors will help generate 
programs and practices that are successful. Thus, one goal of the current study was to begin to 
address this gap by examining two specific protective factors: (1) family routines and (2) general 
parenting.  
The Present Study 
As the literature suggests, family routines may be a critical protective factor for at-risk 
children. Because 19% of all children under 18 in the United States live in poverty, researchers 
must examine protective factors that can offset the detrimental effects of adversity or risk and 
foster resilience in children and families. In addition to conventional general parenting, family 
routines should be explored. The development of a stable and predictable home environment 
may be especially important for low-income children who are dealing with the stressors 
 
 
25 
 
associated with a lack of economic resources (Seaton & Taylor, 2003). However, little is known 
about the role that routines in general and specific routines in particular play in the development 
of very young children. The goal of the present study was to determine whether family routines 
predicted child social and cognitive outcomes beyond general parenting. In addition, the 
possibility of family routines as a moderator of the relationship between general parenting and 
child outcomes was explored. That is, I examined whether the interaction between general 
parenting and family routines significantly predicts child social and cognitive outcomes. Finally, 
I explored whether the effects of family routines differ by child gender and race/ethnicity. More 
specifically, I asked:  
1. Do family routines predict preschoolers? social/behavioral development and cognitive 
ability? Which routines are the strongest predictors? 
2. Do family routines predict preschoolers? social/behavioral development and cognitive 
ability beyond general positive parenting?   
3. Do family routines moderate the relationship between general parenting and child 
outcomes at 36 months? Does child gender and race/ethnicity moderate the relationship 
between family routines and child outcomes at 36 months? 
  
 
 
26 
 
Method 
Sample 
 Data from the Birth to Three Phase (1996-2001) of the Early Head Start Research and 
Evaluation Project (EHSRE) was used in the current study. EHSRE is an ongoing longitudinal, 
17-site evaluation of some of the first federally funded Early Head Start Programs serving low-
income infants, toddlers, and their families (Brooks-Gunn, Berlin, Leventhal, & Fuligni, 2000). 
Early Head Start (EHS) services begin between the third trimester of pregnancy and the target 
child?s 12th month of age and continue through the child?s third year. Sites were selected to 
gather adequate representation of major programmatic approaches and settings as well as a 
diversity of characteristics typical of EHS families nationally (Administration on Children and 
Families, 2002). One additional selection criteria focused on the strength of the local research 
team. The 17 sites recruited 3,001 families. To be eligible for the study, all families had to: (a) 
have incomes near or below the federal poverty level at the time of enrollment; (b) have had a 
child under one year of age born between September 1, 1995 and September 30, 1998; (c) 
understand the random assignment procedures and express willingness to participate in the 
program or control group; and (d) have not participated in an early childhood intervention for at 
least three months. Respondents were randomly assigned to the program (1,513; EHS 
programming included comprehensive child development and parenting education services) or 
comparison group (1,488). Program and comparison groups were equivalent at the start of the 
study. Participants in the comparison group were free to access other services in their 
communities (Bradley, Chazan-Cohen, & Raikes, 2009). 
 
 
27 
 
The current investigation is based on a sample of 1,232 mothers and children who were 
still in the study at three years of age and who had complete data for the child outcomes of 
interest (i.e., social and cognitive outcomes). The sample is diverse with respect to gender 
(51.5% female) and ethnicity (39% African American, 13% Hispanic, and 48% White). 
Approximately 53% of the children were enrolled in the EHS program. Sixty-seven percent of 
mothers had completed high school or earned their GED, 79% were not married or cohabitating, 
and 23% had depression scores on the CES-D high enough to be considered clinical. On average, 
the families? income as a percent of poverty was 60%. Nineteen percent of the mothers reported 
they perceived the family had inadequate money or urgent financial need.  Response rates 
decreased somewhat over time. The 1,745 families that did not have child outcome data at 36 
months varied slightly from those that remained in the study. The mothers were less likely to be 
living alone (34.2%); were more likely to report they perceived the family had inadequate money 
or urgent financial need (23%); and were more likely to have depression scores on the CES-D 
high enough to be considered clinical (50%).  
Procedure 
Sample enrollment, baseline data collection, and random assignment began in July 1996 and 
were completed in September 1998. Program staff collected baseline data from all families (99% 
mothers) during the application and enrollment process (Head Start Family Information System 
Program Application and Enrollment Forms). Families were then randomized to treatment and 
control. Data collection occurred in the home around the children?s 14-month, 24-month, and 36-
month birthdays. Data-collection methods included direct child assessment and parent interview 
by trained data collectors, as well as videotaped observations of mother?child interactions, which 
 
 
28 
 
were coded by an independent research team (Brady-Smith, O?Brien, Berlin, Ware, & Fauth, 
2000) at the Center for Children and Families, Columbia University, Teachers College.  
Measures 
Outcome Variables 
Child social and behavior development. To assess children?s social and behavioral skills at 
36-months of age, the Bayley Behavioral Rating Scale (BRS) and the Child Behavior Checklist 
were used. The BRS is one of three component scales of the Bayley Scales of Infant 
Developemnt-II (Bayley, 1993). At the conclusion of the Mental Development Index (MDI) 
assessment, the interviewer assessed the child?s behavior throughout the assessment using a 5-
point Likert scale, with 5 indicating more positive behaviors. The BRS is composed of two 
subscales: emotion regulation and orientation/engagement. The emotion regulation subscale 
measures a child?s ability to change tasks and test materials and the orientation/engagement 
subscale measures the child?s cooperation with the interviewer as well as interest in the test 
materials. Taken together, the BRS has alpha coefficients ranging from .64 to .92 and thus is 
internally consistent (McIntire & Miller, 2007). In addition, the 19-item aggressive behavior 
subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) was completed 
by the parent. For each item, parents indicated whether the statement describing a particular 
behavior was not true (0), somewhat or sometimes true (1), or very or often true (2) of their 
child. Sample behaviors were ?Child has temper tantrums,? ?Child hits others,? and ?Child is 
easily frustrated.? The CBCL is a widely used measure to assess the social competence and 
problem behavior of children 1 ? to 5 years. The CBCL has excellent concurrent and predictive 
validity. Specifically the measure discriminates well between referred and nonreferred toddlers, 
and has been shown to predict problem behavior over a 6-year period (Achenbach, 1991). 
 
 
29 
 
Child cognitive and language development. To assess children?s cognitive skills at 36-
months of age, two direct assessments were used. The first was the Mental Development Index 
(MDI) subscale of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II (Bayley, 1993). The MDI 
measures overall cognitive development (e.g., perceptual abilities, memory, and problem 
solving) and is normed for children 1 to 42 months of age. The Bayley Scales are the most 
widely used measures of infant cognitive ability and have very good psychometric properties 
(Gagnon & Nagle, 2000). The second measure was the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III 
(PPVT-III), a widely used measure of receptive vocabulary that measures the size and range of 
words that the child understands (Dunn & Dunn, 1997; 2001). The PPVT consist of 130 items in 
which an interviewer says a word (such as ?hand,? ?feather,? and ?arrow?) and asks the child to 
choose ?the best picture of it.? Words become progressively more difficult, and the interviewer 
stops when the child makes six consecutive errors (i.e., a ?ceiling? is reached). PPVT scores in 
early childhood have been shown to be correlated with literacy outcomes in young adulthood 
(Bayadar, Brooks-Gunn, & Furstenberg, 1993). The PPVT is considered a reliable and valid 
measure of a child?s current verbal ability, with split-half reliabilities ranging from .70 to .84 for 
3- to 5-year olds (Dunn & Dunn, 1981). Previous research shows high correlations between both 
of these measures and various concurrent and criterion measures, for example the Wechsler 
Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (Smith et al., 1997; Wechsler, 1991).    
Primary Predictors of Interest 
Family routines. Five observed variables were used to represent the latent construct family 
routines: parent-child play, parent-child outside activities, bedtime routine, regular bedtime, and 
reading routine. Each routine type was assessed through maternal report when the child was 14-, 
24-, and 36-months of age using items appropriate for the child?s developmental period. Parent-
 
 
30 
 
child play and parent-child outside activities were represented by two categorical observed 
variables, respectively. Parents were asked to rate how often they participated in specific play 
(i.e., tell stories, sing songs, dance) and outside activities (i.e., take child with you to religious 
services, visit museum, go on an outing such as shopping, to the park, or on a picnic) with their 
child on a scale from 1 (more than once a day) to 6 (not at all). An average of reverse-coded 
items in each subscale was used such that higher scores reflect higher frequency of activities. 
Bedtime routines was represented by a dichotomous observed variable. Parents were asked, ?Do 
you have a regular routine of things you do with your child when you put him/her to sleep that 
was carried out at least 4 or 5 times in the last week?? A similar question was asked for whether 
the child had a regular bedtime. Parents responded by saying yes (1) or no (0) to all items. 
Reading routine was represented by a dichotomous observed variable. Parents responded yes (1) 
or no (0) to whether they read daily with their child.  
General positive parenting. Three observed variables were used to represent the latent 
construct general positive parenting: discipline severity, language and cognitive stimulation, and 
maternal supportiveness. Discipline severity was assessed at 14, 24, and 36 months through 
maternal self-report on the Index of Severity of Discipline Strategies (Administration on Children 
and Families, 2002). This index measures the degree of harshness of discipline strategies the 
parent suggested. Trained interviewers read four different conflict situations to parents that they 
may have with their child (the child keeps playing with breakable things; the child refuses to eat; 
the child throws a temper tantrum in a public place; the child hits the parent in anger; the last 
situation was only at 36 months) and parents provided open-ended answers to how they would 
respond to each of the four situations. Responses were then classified into types of discipline 
strategies (e.g., time out, warn/remind, threaten the child with punishment, shout at the child, 
 
 
31 
 
physical punishment). Scores can range from 1 (preventing the situation or distracting the child, 
removing the child or object, talking to the child, or putting the child in time out) to 5 (using 
physical punishment). A parent?s final score is represented by the harshest strategy that was 
suggested in response to any of the four conflict situations. Thus, parents who said they would 
use physical punishment receive a 5; those who did not suggest physical punishment but did say 
they would shout at the child receive a 4; those whose harshest response was to threaten the child 
with punishment receive a 3; those who suggest sending the child to his or her room, ignoring the 
behavior, threatening time out or loss of treats, or saying ?No!? receive a 2; and those who 
suggested only preventing the situation or distracting the child, removing the child or object, 
talking to the child, or putting the child in time out receive a 1.  
Language and cognitive stimulation in the home was assessed at 14, 24, and 36 months using 
the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 
1984) Support of Cognitive, Language, and Literacy Subscale. The subscale is the sum of 12 
items that assess the presence of a variety of stimulating materials. Information needed to score 
the inventory was collected through both maternal interviews and observations of the child?s 
home while the child was present. All HOME data collectors were certified at 85% reliability on 
administration and scoring of the HOME at all three time points (Administration on Children, 
Youth, and Families, 2002). Dichotomous items such as ?At least ten books are present and 
visible? and ?Parent talks to child while doing house work? were coded and summed. This total 
score was used in the current analyses. Higher scores indicate home environments that are more 
supportive. The HOME is a well validated and widely used instrument that is consistently found 
to be associated with child outcomes (Bradley, 1989, 1995; Bradley & Caldwell, 1984; Caldwell 
& Bradley, 1984; Menaghan & Parcel, 1991).   
 
 
32 
 
Finally, maternal supportiveness was coded from a semi-structured play task designed to 
capture the degree to which interactions between mothers and children were warm and 
responsive (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1997) when the child was 14, 24, and 
36 months. During the 10 min play task, respondents were given three cloth bags of toys and 
invited to play with their child however they wished as long as they used the bags in a specified 
order. The instructions given for the play task were: ?Our last activity will take about 10 minutes. 
We would like you and (CHILD) to spend this time with the toys in these three bags. During this 
activity, you may play with (CHILD) if you like. Just to remind you, please face front and try to 
stay on the mat. Please start with Bag #1, move on to Bag #2, and finish with Bag #3. Do you 
have any questions?? The instructions were purposefully vague to elicit naturally occurring 
behaviors. Videotapes of dyads interacting were coded according to scales adopted from the 
NICHD Study of Early Child Care?s Three Box coding scales (NICHD Early Child Care 
Research Network, 1997, 1999). Trained observers rated mothers on a variety of behaviors using 
a 7-point Likert scale (1= ?very low? to 7 ?very high?).  
Six parenting scales address parent behaviors, but only three are relevant to this study. The 
three scales address sensitivity (the extent to which the parent takes the child?s perspective, 
accurately perceives the child?s signals, and promptly and appropriately responds to these 
signals); positive regard (demonstration of love, respect, admiration); and stimulation of 
cognitive development (teaching, actively trying to expand the child?s abilities). Parental 
sensitivity, cognitive stimulation, and positive regard were highly correlated and thus a 
composite parenting score reflecting ?supportive? parenting behavior was created by computing 
the mean scores for the three aforementioned subscales. This composite was used in the current 
 
 
33 
 
analyses. Significant positive correlations have been found between supportive parenting and 
HOME inventory scores (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984; Spiker, Ferguson, & Brooks-Gunn, 1993). 
Control Variables 
Child sex. Maternal reports of child sex were collected at study entry. Child sex was 
represented by a dichotomous variable (Male = 1).  
Maternal characteristics. Parent reports of primary caregiver?s race were collected at study 
entry. Race was represented by three dichotomous variables. One variable represented African-
American versus White/Hispanic, one represented Hispanic versus White/African American and 
the final variable represented White versus African-American/Hispanic. Information on maternal 
education level was obtained when children were 14-, 24-, and 36-months of age and combined 
across waves. For the current study, maternal education was represented by a dichotomous 
variable (Has high school diploma/GED = 1). I also controlled for whether the mother was 
married or cohabitating (living alone or with other adults).  
Maternal depressive symptoms was assessed via self-report when children were 14 months 
using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression scale, Short Form (Ross, Mirowsky, & 
Huber, 1983), which consisted of 12 items extracted from the original 20-item instrument 
(Radlof, 1977).  Respondents were asked the number of days in the past week they had a 
particular symptom. Symptoms include poor appetite, restless sleep, loneliness, sadness, and lack 
of energy. Items were coded on a 4-point scale from rarely (0) to most days (3), with higher 
scores representing more depressive symptoms. Scores on the scale range from 0 to 36. Scores of 
greater than or equal to 10 indicate potential clinical depression levels (Chazen-Cohen et al., 
2007). Maternal depressive symptoms was represented by a dichotomous variable (probably 
depressed/score greater than 23 = 1) 
 
 
34 
 
Family characteristics. Family financial information was obtained via maternal interviews 
at baseline. An income-to-needs variable was constructed by dividing the total family income by 
the poverty threshold for a family of that size, as determined by the US Bureau of the Census 
(Administration on Children and Families, 2002). Perceived financial inadequacy (Family has 
inadequate amount of money = 1) as reported by the mother at baseline was also used to capture 
both objective and subjective experiences with poverty.  
Program status. I included a dummy variable representing EHS program status (Program = 
1) to control for program effects.  
  
 
 
35 
 
Results 
Analysis Plan 
Exploratory analyses. I conducted descriptive statistics to obtain the means and standard 
deviations of the outcome and predictor variables for the total analytic sample, as well as 
separately by child gender and maternal ethnicity. In addition, I conducted individual t-tests to 
determine whether statistically significant differences exist between sample means for boys and 
girls, and I used ANOVAs to test pairwise differences across Hispanics, African Americans, and 
Whites. The outcome and predictor variables were symmetric enough (e.g., no skewness or 
kurtosis values greater than +/- 2), thus transformation of these variables was not necessary. 
Additionally, I examined bivariate correlations between all outcome and predictor variables.  
Next, to test whether the hypothesized indicators of family routines (i.e., bedtime, play, and 
reading) adequately measured this construct, I first estimated two separate confirmatory factor 
analysis (CFA) models examining whether the indicators bedtime routine and regular bedtime 
represented bedtime routines and then whether parent-child play and parent-child outside 
activities represented play routines. The factor loadings for bedtime routine and parent-child play 
were fixed to one, for bedtime and play routines respectively, to provide the scaling unit. Model 
fit was poor for both latent constructs, so I re-fit the measurement model using the five indictors 
of routines as indices of a single latent construct representing overall family routines. I then 
tested whether the hypothesized indicators of general parenting (i.e., discipline severity, 
language and cognitive stimulation, and maternal supportiveness) adequately measured these 
constructs. 
 
 
36 
 
Finally, to test whether the hypothesized social competence indicators (i.e., 
orientation/engagement, emotion regulation, and aggressive behavior) and cognitive ability 
indicators (i.e., MDI and PPVT ) adequately reflect children?s social and cognitive outcomes, I 
fit a set of measurement models again using CFA. The model for the cognitive latent construct 
failed to identify or produce reliable estimates of the factor loadings, so using factor analysis I 
created a single composite variable to reflect overall cognitive ability at 36 months (? = .72). 
The fit of these measurement models to my data were examined using goodness of fit indices. I 
considered model fit to be good if I obtained a non-significant Chi-Square, Comparative Fit 
Indices (CFI) that were between .90 and 1, and a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation 
(RMSEA) that was close to zero with a non-significant p-value. 
Next I fit a set of latent growth curve models to reveal within-individual differences in family 
routines and general parenting between 14 and 36 months. I selected latent growth curve analysis 
(LGCA) over other more traditional growth modeling methods because it offers several well-
documented advantages (Muthen & Curran, 1997; Willet & Bub, 2004). First, LGCA allows 
change in two domains (i.e., family routines and parenting practices) to be modeled 
simultaneously. Further, LGCA models can be specified so that trajectories of change over time 
in family routines and parenting practices are predictors of subsequent outcomes (i.e., social and 
cognitive skills at 36 months), rather than being treated as a outcomes themselves (Bub, 
McCartney, & Willett, 2007). Importantly, LGCA incorporates a ?measurement model? that 
teases out error from the observation of family routines and parenting practices over time 
(Willett & Sayer, 1994).  
I hypothesized that the measurement models would link the longitudinal assessments of 
either family routines or general parenting to latent constructs that represent the family?s true 
 
 
37 
 
final status in routines or parenting (?0i) at 36 months and their true linear rate of change in these 
domains between 14 and 36 months (?1i; See Figure 1). Factor loadings were fixed and equal to a 
constant of 1 to represent true initial status or to the values of the times at which the routines and 
practices were measured (i.e., 14, 24, and 36 months), centered on the last assessment to 
represent true linear rate of change in these domains. I centered time at 36 months because I was 
more concerned with the concurrent effects of family routines on child outcomes than how base 
level routines influence later development. Results suggested there was no statistically 
significant growth in family routines or general parenting over time; thus, for all subsequent 
analyses I used the 14- , 24- , and 36-month family routines and general parenting latent 
constructs developed in the aforementioned measurement models to predict outcomes at 36 
months (see Figure 2).  
Predictive analyses. To address my first research question of whether family routines 
predict preschoolers? social/behavioral skills and cognitive ability, I fit a taxonomy of 
hierarchical multiple regression models in which I regressed children?s social and cognitive 
outcomes first on the latent construct representing 14-month family routines, then on the latent 
constructs representing 14- and 24-month family routines, and finally on the latent constructs 
representing 14-, 24-, and 36-month family routines. In total, six regression models were fitted: 
three for social outcomes and three for cognitive outcomes. I also included a common set of 
control variables in each model: child sex, caregiver?s race, maternal education, live-in partner 
status, maternal depression, the family?s income-to-needs ratio, maternal perceived financial 
inadequacy, and program status. A sample equation from these models is: 
Yi = ?0 + ?1Routines_14 + ?2X                                      (Equation 1) 
 
 
38 
 
where ?0 represents the intercept when all else in the model is zero, ?1 represents the slope of 
routines (in this example at 14 months) and ?2 represents the slope of the vector of covariates.  
Additionally, to examine whether certain family routines more strongly predicted children?s 
social or cognitive outcomes, I fit a taxonomy of multiple regression models in which I regressed 
children?s social or cognitive outcomes on each of the five observed family routine variables at 
36 months (i.e., parent-child play, parent-child outside activities, bedtime routine, regular 
bedtime, and reading routine). More specifically, I examined the effects of parent-child play, 
parent-child outside activities, bedtime routine, regular bedtime, and reading routine on the latent 
construct representing children?s 36-month social competence as well as on the observed 
composite score representing children?s 36-month cognitive skills. In total, 10 regression models 
were fitted: five for social outcomes and five for cognitive outcomes. Again, I included a 
common set of control variables in these models. 
To address my second research question of whether family routines predict child outcomes 
above and beyond general parenting, I added to the first set of models described above the latent 
constructs representing general parenting practices at 14, 24, and 36 months. More specifically, 
to the model containing 14-month family routines, I added 14-month general parenting; to the 
model containing both the 14- and 24-month family routines constructs, I added the 14- and 24-
month general parenting constructs; and finally, to the model containing 14-, 24- and 36-month 
family routines constructs, I added the 14-, 24- and 36-month general parenting constructs. In 
total, six regression models were fitted: three for social outcomes and three for cognitive 
outcomes. I again included in each model the common set of control variables described above. 
A sample equation from these models is: 
Yi = ?0 + ?1Routines_14 + ?2Parenting_14 +  ?3X                       (Equation 2) 
 
 
39 
 
where ?0 represents the intercept when all else in the model is zero, ?1 represents the slope of 
routines (in this example at 14 months), ?2 represents the slope of the general parenting (in this 
example at 14 months), and ?3 represents the slope of the vector of covariates. Model fit of these 
models was compared to the model fit of the models with only family routines as the predictor 
variables using delta chi square statistics. 
Finally, to address my third research question of whether family routines moderated the 
relationship between general parenting and child outcomes at 36 months and whether these 
relationships varied by child gender and race/ethnicity, I added an interaction between the family 
routines and general parenting latent constructs separately at each age. The models did not 
identify so I next tested the interaction between each observed family routines variable and the 
observed general parenting variables. In total, 90 additional regression models were fitted. These 
included an observed family routine, an observed general parenting variable, and the interaction 
between an observed family routine and observed general parenting variable at either 14, 24, or 
36 months as the predictor variables and either social or cognitive outcomes as the outcome 
variable.  
Additionally, to examine whether child gender and race/ethnicity moderate the relationship 
between family routines and child outcomes at 36 months, I conducted multi-group analyses 
using the models from my first research question (i.e., those with just routines predicting 
children?s social and cognitive outcomes). First, I constrained all of the paths (i.e., factor 
loadings for the measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, and structural 
parameters linking routines to child outcomes) to be fixed and equal across gender or 
race/ethnicity separately by age and outcome. Next, I fit a fully unconstrained model in which 
the factor loadings for the measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables and 
 
 
40 
 
structural parameters linking routines to child outcomes were allowed to be freely estimated, 
separately by age and outcome. The model fit for the unconstrained models was significantly 
better than that for the constrained models for both gender and race/ethnicity suggesting that 
there are differences in the models for girls and boys, and European American, African 
American, and Hispanics, respectively. Therefore, I fit a model allowing the structural 
parameters linking routines to child outcomes to be free and the measurement models to remain 
fixed and equal across gender and race/ethnicity separately by age and outcome. These models 
explored whether the differences in the model fit are due to the effects of routines on child 
outcomes differing by child gender or race/ethnicity. Finally, I fit an unconstrained model 
allowing the measurement models to be estimated freely and the parameters linking routines to 
child outcomes to remain fixed and equal across gender and race/ethnicity separately by age and 
outcome. These models explored whether the differences in the model fit are attributable to 
differences in the measurement models and not the regression parameters, as originally 
hypothesized.  
All analyses were conducted using MPlus version 5.0 and SPSS 18. Model fit was evaluated 
by a non-significant Chi-Square, Comparative Fit Indices (CFI) that were between .90 and 1, and 
a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) that was close to zero with a non-
significant p-value. Missing data on the key predictors was handled using Full Information 
Maxiumum Likelihood procedures, which is the default in MPlus (Muth?n & Muth?n, 1998 -
2007).  
Preliminary Analyses 
Descriptive statistics. Sample means and standard deviations for the outcome, key predictor, 
and control variables are presented in Table 1. Children displayed relatively high levels of 
 
 
41 
 
emotion regulation and orientation/engagement at 36 months, as evidenced by average scores of 
4.01 and 3.93, respectively, with possible scores ranging from 1 to 5. On average, aggressive 
behavior scores as rated by caregivers were within the normal range (Achenbach, 1991). 
Children displayed relatively low to moderate cognitive abilities, as evidenced by an average 
score of 91.33 on the MDI and 83.85 on the PPVT, both below the normed values of 100. 
However, there was substantial variability around the mean, as indexed by a standard deviation 
of just under 13 for the MDI and just under 15 for the PPVT. Outcomes varied by gender, with 
girls obtaining higher social and cognitive scores than boys (see Table 2). Outcomes also varied 
by race (see Table 3). Ethnic minority children performed lower than majority children on all 
outcomes, including maternal reports of aggressive behavior. 
On average, over half of the sample participated in some sort of family routine between 14 
and 36 months, with bedtime routines being the most common (see Table 1). Parents, on 
average, reported low levels of parent-child outside activity as evidenced by average scores of 
2.62 at 14 months to 2.89 at 36 months, with possible scores ranging from 1 to 6. Higher levels 
of general parent-child play were reported with an average score of 4.51 across all three time 
points. In general, parents reported using mild discipline techniques. However, over time, 
discipline scores steadily increased, indicating that as children grew older parents tended to use 
more harsh discipline strategies. Observations also indicated that maternal supportiveness was 
high as evidenced by average scores of 4.00 at 36 months to 4.04 at 14 months, with possible 
scores ranging from 1 to 7.  However, family routines and general parenting values differed from 
occasion to occasion. Over time, scores steadily decreased for regular bedtime and increased for 
parent-child outside activities. That is, fewer children had a regular bedtime routine as they got 
older and parents and children engaged in more outside play as children aged. Similarly, scores 
 
 
42 
 
seemed to increase for language and cognitive stimulation but decrease for maternal 
supportiveness. Reading routines, parent-child play, and language and cognitive stimulation 
varied by gender with families with girls reporting higher levels of the three constructs than 
families with boys (see Table 2). In addition, all of the family routine and general parenting 
variables differed by race (see Table 3). Ethnic minority parents reported lower levels of bedtime 
routines, regular bedtime, reading routines, and parent-child play; however, on average, both 
African-American and Hispanic parents reported higher levels of parent-child outside activities 
compared to European-American parents. In addition, African American parents reported using 
harsher discipline strategies than Hispanics and European Americans. Ethnic minority parents 
also scored lower than majority parents on maternal supportiveness and language and cognitive 
stimulation. 
Estimated intercorrelations among the outcome, key predictor, and control variables 
separately by age are presented in Tables 4 to 6. Social skills and cognitive ability at 36 months, 
the two outcomes of interest, were moderately and positively correlated with family routines and 
general parenting within each assessment across time (ranging from r = .06 for bedtime routine 
at 24 and 36 months to .37 for language and cognitive stimulation at 36 months.). Thus, on 
average, children with more family routines and more positive general parenting had more 
positive social skills and higher cognitive abilities when compared to their peers in families with 
fewer routines and less positive general parenting. There was some evidence of intraindividual 
stability in both family routines and general parenting, as demonstrated by the moderate positive 
correlations among observed family routine or general parenting variables at 14, 24, and 36 
months (see Table 7). More specifically, parents appear to maintain their rank order of family 
routines and general parenting between 14 and 36 months. 
 
 
43 
 
Measurement models. Five observed variables were used to represent the latent construct 
family routines separately by age: parent-child play, parent-child outside activities, bedtime 
routine, regular bedtime, and reading routine (see Figure 3). At 14, 24 and 36 months of age the 
factor loading for bedtime routines was fixed to one to provide the scaling unit. Each of the latent 
constructs (i.e., family routines at 14 months, family routines at 24 months, and family routines 
at 36 months), the observed variables across age (e.g., parent-child play at 14 months with 
parent-child play at 24 months), and the same domain observed variables within each latent 
construct (i.e., parent-child play with parent-child outside activities, and bedtime routine with 
regular bedtime), respectively, were allowed to covary. Each of the observed variables 
significantly loaded onto the family routines factor at each age; however, they did not contribute 
to the construct equally within age (see Table 8). More specifically, parent-child play contributed 
the most to the construct at each age (with standardized factor loadings ranging from a low of .78 
at 14 months to a high of .89 at 24 months). Reading routines also contributed strongly to the 
construct (with standardized factor loadings ranging from a low of .65 at 36 months to a high of 
.69 at 24 months). Regular bedtime contributed the least to the construct at all three ages (with 
standardized factor loadings ranging from .20 at 24 months and .22 at 14 months). Examination 
of the fit statistics indicates the model fits the data well (?2 = 189.15, df = 66, p = .00; CFI = .97; 
RMSEA = .039, p = .99). Thus, a single latent construct at each age was used to represent overall 
family routines.  
I used three observed variables to represent the latent construct general parenting: discipline 
severity, language and cognitive stimulation, and maternal supportiveness (see Figure 4 and 
Table 9). At 14, 24 and 36 months of age the factor loading for maternal supportiveness was 
fixed to one to provide the scaling unit. Each of the latent constructs (i.e., parenting at 14 
 
 
44 
 
months, parenting at 24 months, and parenting at 36 months) as well as the observed variables 
across age (e.g., maternal supportiveness at 14 months with maternal supportiveness at 24 
months) were allowed to covary. The two observed variables that were freely estimated 
significantly loaded onto the latent construct representing general parenting. At each age, 
language and cognitive stimulation contributed the most to the construct (with standardized 
factor loadings ranging from a low of .46 at 14 months to a high of .60 at 36 months); however, 
discipline severity also contributed strongly to the construct (with standardized factor loadings 
ranging from -.46 at 36 months to -.30 at 14 months). Examination of the fit statistics indicates 
the measurement model fits the data perfectly (?2 = 13.53, df = 15, p = .00; CFI = 1.0;  
RMSEA = .00, p = 1.0). Thus, a single latent constructing representing general positive parenting 
at 14, 24, or 36 months was retained for subsequent analyses. 
Finally, three observed variables were used to represent the latent construct of social 
competence at 36 months: emotion regulation, orientation/engagement, and aggressive behavior 
(see Figure 5). The factor loading for orientation/engagement was fixed to one to provide the 
scaling unit. Aggressive behavior and emotion regulation significantly loaded on the factor with 
standardized factor loadings of -.16 (p < .001) and .88 (p < .001), respectively. Emotion 
regulation contributed the most to the construct, evidenced by the largest standardized factor 
loading as well as the R2 statistic (R2 = .77) (see Table 10). As is commonly the case with 
measurement models consisting of a limited number of variables, the model was fully saturated, 
evidenced by a CFI of 1 and a RMSEA of 0. Thus, a single latent construct representing social 
competence at 36 months was retained for all subsequent analyses. The measurement model 
representing cognitive outcomes (i.e., MDI and PPVT) did not fit the data well (failed to 
identify), so I created a single composite variable representing overall cognitive ability (? = .72) 
 
 
45 
 
using the MDI and PPVT scores. This poor fit was likely due to the fact that I only had two 
observed variables.  
I also tested whether the measurement models, taken together, were the same across time by 
fitting a model in which I constrained the loadings for each observed variable to be the same at 
each age. I conducted a delta chi square test and found a delta chi square of 906.41 (27) between 
the fully constrained and fully unconstrained models. Therefore, the fully unconstrained model 
provides a better model fit (?2 = 540.14, df = 207, p = .00; CFI = .95; RMSEA = .036, p = 1.00), 
which suggests that routines and general parenting look different across time. Thus, the 
unconstrained measurement model was retained for subsequent analyses.  
Research Question 1: Do family routines predict preschoolers? social/behavioral 
development and cognitive ability? Which routines are the strongest predictors? Parameter 
estimates and goodness of fit statistics from the models addressing my first research question are 
presented in Tables 11 and 12, Models 1 through 3. Family routines positively and statistically 
significantly predicted both children?s social competence (Table 11) and cognitive ability (Table 
12) at 36 months. More specifically, children in families that reported higher levels of routines at 
14 months (see Model 1) had higher levels of social competence at 36 months, suggesting there 
may be a long-term effect of family routines on children?s social competence. Child gender was 
a significant covariate, with females having significantly higher social competence scores than 
males, further supporting the need to examine gender differences across models. The majority of 
the remaining covariates, including race ethnicity, were not significant. When 24-month family 
routines were added to the model (see Model 2, Table 11), the effect of 14-month family routines 
declined but remained statistically significant and there was improved model fit. In addition, 
when controlling for 14-month routines, family routines at 24 months positively and significantly 
 
 
46 
 
predicted children?s social competence at 36 months. This again suggests there may be long-term 
effects of early routines on children?s social competence. Importantly, the effect may be additive, 
as evidenced by the fact that both 14- and 24-month routines were statistically significant when 
considered simultaneously. Finally, when the 36-month family routines construct was added to 
the model, the effects of family routines at 14 and 24 months became non-significant (i.e., zero) 
but the 36-month family routines construct positively and significantly predicted children?s 
social competence at 36 months. That is, children in families with more routines at 36 months 
also had higher social competence scores at 36 months. This indicates that while early routines 
appear to matter for later child outcomes when considered separately from concurrent effects, the 
concurrent effects of 36-month family routines on social competence may outweigh the early 
effects when all three constructs are considered simultaneously. Model 3 (Table 11), which 
included family routines at all three time points, offered an improved fit over Model 2  
(?2 = 1311.37, df = 456, p = .00; CFI = .88; RMSEA = .039, p = 1.00). 
I re-fit these models using the observed cognitive composite I created as the outcome 
variable instead of the social competence latent construct. The pattern of findings was similar to 
that found for social competence, with two exceptions. First, the covariates, including child 
gender and race/ethnicity, more strongly predicted children?s 36-month cognitive ability, such 
that females had significantly higher cognitive ability scores than males, and ethnic minorities 
had significantly lower cognitive ability scores when compared to ethnic majority children 
(Table 12). Second, there were not concurrent effects of 36-month family routines on children?s 
cognitive ability when the 14- and 24-month constructs were included. Conversely, when 36-
month family routines were added to the model, 14- and 24-month family routines continued to 
positively and significantly predict children?s 36-month cognitive ability and 36-month family 
 
 
47 
 
routines were not a significant predictor. This suggests that while concurrent routines may be 
critical for fostering social competence at 36 months, early family routines may be more 
important for predicting children?s 36-month cognitive ability. Model 3 (Table 12), which 
included family routines at all three time points, offered an improved fit over Model 2  
(?2 = 1220.49, df = 390, p = .00; CFI = .89; RMSEA = .042, p = 1.00).  
Finally, to investigate which specific routines at 36 months might best predict children?s 
social and cognitive outcomes at 36 months, each of the observed indicators of family routines 
(i.e., parent-child play, parent-child outside activities, bedtime routine, regular bedtime, and 
reading routine) was included in a set of hierarchical regression models. Although some routines 
appear to be more salient than others both within and across domains (e.g., Parent-child outside 
activity predicted social competence but not cognitive skills at 36 months and reading routines 
strongly predicted cognitive skills at 36 months but not social skills in the final model), overall 
the results suggest that it is routines as a whole rather than independent family routines that 
contribute the most to children?s social competence at 36 months. More specifically, bedtime 
routines, regular bedtime, reading routines, and parent-child outside activity, but not parent-child 
play, contributed to children?s social outcomes at 36 months. Controlling for additional routine 
types, the coefficients for bedtime routine, regular bedtime, reading routines, and parent-child 
play decline across models, again indicating that it is the routines as a whole that matter the 
most. Parent-child outside activities, with the largest coefficient of all models, appeared to be the 
most salient routine for children?s social outcomes (see Model 5, Table 13). Parent-child play, on 
the other hand, was the least salient routine when predicting social outcomes, and when added to 
the model did not significantly improve model fit. Even within one family, parent-child play can 
look different from day to day and week to week, making it difficult to accurately measure.  
 
 
48 
 
As expected, the pattern of findings for children?s cognitive outcomes differed somewhat 
from children?s social outcomes. Bedtime routine and reading routines, but not regular bedtime, 
parent-child play, and parent-child outside activity, contributed to children?s cognitive outcomes 
at 36 months. Reading routines, with the largest coefficient and best model fit, most strongly 
predicted children?s cognitive outcomes at 36 months (Model 8, Table 13). Even when parent-
child play and parent-child outside activities were added to the model, the coefficient for reading 
routines did not decline. Thus, for cognitive outcomes, the group of routines appears less 
important than then the independent reading routine. 
Research Question 2: Do family routines predict preschoolers? social/behavioral 
development and cognitive ability beyond general positive parenting?  Parameter estimates 
and goodness of fit statistics from the models addressing my second research question are 
presented in Table 11 (social) and 12 (cognitive), Models 4 through 6. Unexpectedly, when 14-
month general parenting was added to the 14-month family routine model, the coefficient for 14-
month family routines remained statistically significant but became negative for both social 
competence and cognitive ability, suggesting that family routines and general parenting as 
measured in this study may be highly collinear constructs and thus their simultaneous inclusion 
produced this pattern of results. However, adding general parenting to the model did lead to 
better overall model fit for both social competence and cognitive ability (social, ?2 = 1267.75,  
df = 457, p = .00; CFI = .89; RMSEA = .038, p = 1.00; cognitive, ?2 = 1075.70, df = 391, p = .00; 
CFI = .91; RMSEA = .038, p = 1.00), suggesting that general parenting matters for child 
outcomes, controlling for family routines. It is likely that family routines represent just one 
aspect of the general parenting indicators added to the models, thus when general parenting is 
added to the model the overall representation of parenting becomes stronger, and the model fit 
 
 
49 
 
better.  When 24-month general parenting was added to the model containing both 14- and 24-
month family routines constructs, all of the coefficients on the key predictors (i.e., family 
routines at 14 and 24 months and general parenting at 14 and 24 month) for both social 
competence and cognitive ability, became negative and non-significant (see Tables 11 and 12). 
This pattern of findings continued when 36-month family routines were added to the model. This 
provides further evidence that the routines and parenting constructs are likely highly collinear, 
measuring the same (or a similar) underlying domain of parenting. Importantly, when 36-month 
general parenting was added to the model, model fit became worse for both social competence 
and cognitive abilities (social, ?2 = 1427.91, df = 454, p = .00; CFI = .87; RMSEA = .038,  
p = 1.00; cognitive, ?2 = 1243.16, df = 388, p = .00; CFI = .88; RMSEA = .042, p = 1.00). Thus 
the final models for social competence and cognitive ability do not include 36-month family 
routines and general parenting constructs, but rather only 14- and 24-month constructs. It is 
worth noting that these are highly conservative models, particularly Model 6 with high 
correlations between constructs across time. The overlap among the latent constructs for family 
routines and general parenting does not permit proper interpretation of the coefficients because 
the constructs are likely explaining the same variation in the outcome. 
Research Question 3: Do family routines moderate the relationship between general 
parenting and child outcomes at 36 months? Does child gender and race/ethnicity moderate 
the relationship between family routines and child outcomes at 36 months? Although I fit a 
series of models testing interactions between the latent constructs representing family routines 
and those representing general parenting, the models failed to identify or produce reliable 
estimates of the interaction coefficients. I then tested interactions between the five observed 
family routine variables and the three observed general parenting variables. All interactions were 
 
 
50 
 
non-significant. Thus, it does not appear that the effect of general parenting on children?s social 
and cognitive outcomes differs by family routines. This is not especially surprising given the 
considerable overlap in the domains/practices measured. As found in Research Question 2, when 
both family routines and general parenting were in the model simultaneously, unexpected results 
were obtained because the constructs are highly correlated. It is difficult to tease apart and 
identify the unique contributions of family routines from general parenting as they are measured 
in this dataset due to this high collinearity.   
Differences by child gender. Unstandardized parameter estimates and goodness of fit 
statistics for the multi-group analyses are presented in Tables 14 (social) and 15 (cognitive). 
Models 1, 5 and 9 reflect the constrained models (i.e., factor loadings for measurement models, 
intercepts for the observed variables, and structural parameters linking routines to child 
outcomes were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 months respectively. 
Models 2, 6 and 10 reflect the regression unconstrained models (i.e., structural parameters 
linking routines to child outcomes were freely estimated but the factor loadings for the 
measurement models were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 months 
respectively. Models 3, 7 and 11 reflect the measurement unconstrained models (i.e., factor 
loadings for the measurement models were freely estimated but the structural parameters linking 
routines to child outcomes were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 
months respectively. Finally, Models 4, 8, and 12 reflect the fully unconstrained models (i.e., 
factor loadings for measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, and structural 
parameters linking routines to child outcomes were freely estimated) at 14 months, 24, and 36 
months respectively. 
 
 
51 
 
Family routines at 14 months. In the model in which I constrained the parameters of 
interest (i.e., factor loadings for the measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, 
and structural parameters linking routines to child outcomes) to be fixed and equal across girls 
and boys, 14-month family routines positively and significantly predicted both girls? and boys? 
36-month social competence (see Model 1, Table 14) and cognitive ability (see Model 1, Table 
15). Standardized coefficients (not presented in the tables) suggest that the effect size for social 
competence and cognitive skills was approximately equal for girls and boys (.26 (p < .001) and 
.24 (p < .001) for social competence and .23 (p < .001) and .22 (p <. 001) for cognitive ability). 
Model fit statistics suggest that the constrained model provides a relatively poor fit to the data, as 
indexed by the high chi-square statistics and the low CFI statistics.  
Next I allowed the main parameter of interest, the structural parameter linking routines to 
child outcomes, to be freely estimated across groups but the factor loadings for the measurement 
models were fixed to be equal across groups. Family routines at 14 months had a modest effect 
on both girls? and boys? social competence (see Model 2, Table 14 and Figure 6) but the effect 
was much larger for girls than for boys (standardized coefficient was .32 (p < .10) for girls and 
.18 (p < .05) for boys). The effect of 14-month routines on children?s cognitive skills (see Model 
2, Table 15 and Figure 7) was slightly larger for girls than for boys (i.e., standardized 
coefficients were .24 (p < .001) for girls and .21 (p < .001) for boys). A comparison of the model 
fit statistics between the fully constrained and regression unconstrained models indicated that the 
latter model provided a better fit to the data. On average, girls appear to benefit more from 14-
month family routines than do boys. Specifically, girls whose families have more routines at 14 
months demonstrated better social and cognitive outcomes than did boys whose families has 
similar routines.  
 
 
52 
 
Given that the measurement model for family routines looked somewhat different across 
time (described above), I also tested the possibility that the gender differences I identified in the 
structural pathways were attributable to differences in the measurement models across girls and 
boys. That is, routines could look very different for girls than for boys and thus differences in the 
structural paths might reflect differences in what routines looks like for each group rather than 
the association between family routines and child outcomes per se. In fact, constraining the 
regression parameters to be equal across groups but allowing the measurement models to be 
freely estimated provided a better fit to the data than the fully constrained model for both social 
competence (see Model 3, Table 14) and cognitive ability (see Model 3, Table 15). This suggests 
that, on average, routines may look very different for girls and boys. Investigation of the means 
of family routines at 14 months indicates that on average, families with boys tend to have 
significantly fewer family routines than families with girls, as evidenced by the negative and 
statistically significant coefficients for the 14-month means. In addition, the factor loadings for 
the 14-month family routines construct indicate that while the contribution of each observed 
variable was the same for girls and boys (e.g., parent-child play had the highest factor loading 
and regular bedtime the lowest for both groups), the relative magnitude of the loadings differed 
across groups such that the loadings were stronger for girls (e.g., parent-child play had a loading 
of 5.67 (p < .001) for girls and 3.77(p < .001) for boys).  A comparison of the model fit statistics 
between the fully constrained model (Model 1) and measurement free (Model 3) models 
indicated that the model in which the measurement models were freely estimated and the 
regression was fixed provided a better fit to the data than the fully constrained model. This 
suggests that, on average, families with girls tend to have more routines at 14 months than 
 
 
53 
 
families with boys and thus group differences may simply reflect differences in the quantity of 
routines rather than the relative importance of routines for each outcome.  
Finally, I allowed all of the parameters of interest, including factor loadings for the 
measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, and structural parameters linking 
routines to child outcomes, to be freely estimated across groups. Family routines at 14 months 
had a modest effect on both girls? and boys? social competence (see Model 4, Table 14) but the 
effect was much larger for girls than for boys (standardized coefficient was .32 (p < .05) for girls 
and .19 (p < .05) for boys). In contrast, the effect of 14-month routines on girls? and boys? 
cognitive skills (see Model 4, Table 15) were approximately the same (standardized coefficients 
were .24 (p < .10) for girls and .21 (p < .001) for boys), suggesting that child gender likely 
moderates the link between family routines at 14 months and children?s 36-month social 
competence but not their cognitive skills. Regardless of the size of the effect, it is important to 
note that family routines at 14 months was a consistent positive predictor of children?s social 
competence and cognitive ability across groups. A comparison of the model fit statistics between 
the model allowing only the measurement models to be freely estimated (Model 3) and the fully 
unconstrained model (Model 4) indicated that the model in which all parameters were freely 
estimated provided the best fit to the data. This suggests that, on average, the link between 
family routines at 14 months and children?s social competence at 36 months is stronger for girls 
than for boys partly because girls? families tend to have more routines than boys? families.  
Family routines at 24 and 36 months. A similar taxonomy of models was fitted for 
family routines at 24 and 36 months and the findings were comparable with four exceptions. 
First, converse to the 14-month results, the effects of family routines at 24 or 36 months on 
children?s social competence was much larger for boys than for girls (standardized coefficient 
 
 
54 
 
was .20 (p < .05) for girls and .27 (p < .10) for boys at 24-months; .22 (p < .05) for girls and .36 
(p < .001) for boys at 36-months), in the model allowing the regression parameter to be freely 
estimated (see Figure 6). This suggests that while early routines may be especially important for 
girls? outcomes, later routines seem to be more salient for boys. Second, the effect of 24-month 
routines on children?s cognitive skills was the same for boys and girls (standardized coefficients 
were .24 (p < .001) for girls and .24 (p < .001) for boys) in both the model allowing the 
regression parameter to be freely estimated and the fully unconstrained model. This suggests, 
unlike the 14-month family routines, that child gender likely moderates the link between family 
routines at 24 months and children?s 36-month social skills but not their cognitive skills. Third, 
the effect of 36-month routines on children?s cognitive skills in the fully unconstrained model 
(see Model 12, Table 15) was much larger for boys than for girls (i.e., standardized coefficients 
were .16 (p < .10) for girls and .26 (p < .10) for boys), suggesting that child gender likely 
moderates the link between family routines at 36 months and children?s 36-month social and 
cognitive skills, while gender only moderates the link between family routines at 14 months and 
children?s social competence. Finally, converse to 14-month family routines, the best fitting 
model for 24- and 36-month family routines is the model allowing only the measurement models 
to be freely estimated. This suggests that, on average, differences in routines at 24 and 36 months 
are more salient than the difference in the effect of the routines for children?s social competence 
and cognitive ability at 36 months. 
Differences by race/ethnicity. Unstandardized parameter estimates and goodness of fit 
statistics for the multi-group analyses are presented in Tables 16 (social) and 17 (cognitive).  
Models 1, 5 and 9 reflect the constrained models (i.e., factor loadings for measurement models, 
intercepts for the observed variables, and structural parameters linking routines to child 
 
 
55 
 
outcomes were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 months respectively. 
Models 2, 6 and 10 reflect the regression unconstrained models (i.e., structural parameters 
linking routines to child outcomes were freely estimated but the factor loadings for the 
measurement models were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 months 
respectively. Models 3, 7 and 11 reflect the measurement unconstrained models (i.e., factor 
loadings for the measurement models were freely estimated but the structural parameters linking 
routines to child outcomes were fixed to be equal across groups) at 14 months, 24, and 36 
months respectively. Finally, Models 4, 8, and 12 reflect the fully unconstrained models (i.e., 
factor loadings for measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, and structural 
parameters linking routines to child outcomes were freely estimated) at 14 months, 24, and 36 
months respectively. 
Family routines at 14 months. In the model in which I constrained the parameters of 
interest (i.e., factor loadings for the measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, 
and structural parameters linking routines to child outcomes) to be fixed and equal across 
European-American, African-American, and Hispanic children, 14-month family routines 
positively and significantly predicted European-American, African-American, and Hispanic 
children?s 36-month social competence (see Model 1, Table 16) and cognitive ability (see Model 
1, Table 17). Standardized coefficients (not presented in the tables) suggest that the effect size 
for social competence and cognitive skills was approximately equal for European-American, 
African-American, and Hispanic children (.21 (p < .001), .36 (p < .001), and .18 (p < .001) for 
social competence and .23 (p < .001), .26 (p < .001), and .21(p < .001) for cognitive ability). 
Model fit statistics suggest that the constrained model provides a relatively poor fit to the data, as 
indexed by the high chi-square statistics and the low CFI statistics.  
 
 
56 
 
Next I allowed the main parameter of interest, the structural parameter linking routines to 
child outcomes, to be freely estimated but the factor loadings for the measurement models were 
fixed to be equal across groups. Family routines at 14 months had a modest effect on both 
European-American and African-American children?s social competence (see Model 2, Table 16 
and Figure 8) but no effect on Hispanic children?s social competence. The effect was much 
larger for African Americans than for European Americans (standardized coefficient was  
.39 (p < .10) for African Americans and .20 (p < .05) for European Americans). In contrast, the 
effect of 14-month routines on children?s cognitive skills was approximately the same for 
European-American and African-American children (i.e., standardized coefficients were  
.27 (p < .001) for European Americans and .29 (p < .001) for African Americans), suggesting 
that child race/ethnicity likely moderates the link between family routines at 14 months and 
children?s 36-month social skills but not their cognitive skills (see Figure 9). Family routines at 
14 months had no effect on Hispanic children?s cognitive ability. A comparison of the model fit 
statistics between the fully constrained and regression unconstrained models indicated that the 
latter model provided a better fit to the data. On average, African Americans appear to benefit 
more from 14-month family routines than do European Americans and Hispanics. Specifically, 
African Americans whose families have more routines at 14 months demonstrated better social 
and cognitive outcomes than did European Americans and Hispanics whose families has similar 
routines.  
I also tested the possibility that the race/ethnicity differences I identified in the structural 
pathways were attributable to differences in the measurement models across European 
Americans, African Americans, and Hispanics. Constraining the regression parameters to be 
equal across groups but allowing the measurement models to be freely estimated provided a 
 
 
57 
 
better fit to the data than the fully constrained model for both social competence (see Model 3, 
Table 16) and cognitive ability (see Model 3, Table 17). This suggests that, on average, routines 
look very different for European Americans, African Americans, and Hispanics. However, 
investigation of the means of family routines at 14 months indicates that there are no differences 
in the measurement models across European Americans, African Americans, and Hispanics. In 
addition, the factor loadings for the 14-month family routines construct indicated that the 
contribution of each observed variable was the same for European Americans, African 
Americans, and Hispanics (e.g., parent-child play had the highest factor loading and regular 
bedtime the lowest for all three groups), and the relative magnitude of the loadings was similar 
across groups.  A comparison of the model fit statistics between the fully constrained (Model 1) 
and measurement free (Model 3) models indicated that the model in which the measurement 
models were freely estimated and the regression was fixed provided a better fit to the data than 
the fully constrained model. However, the measurement models do no differ by race/ethnicity. 
The model may fit better simply because any model where paths are freely estimated tends to fit 
better than a fully constrained model as well as because of the presence of the additional 
measurement models (for 24- and 36-month routines) in the overall model.  
Finally, I allowed all of the parameters of interest, including factor loadings for the 
measurement models, intercepts for the observed variables, and structural parameters linking 
routines to child outcomes, to be freely estimated. Family routines at 14 months had a modest 
effect on both European Americans? and African Americans? social competence (see Model 4, 
Table 16) but the effect was much larger for African Americans than for European Americans 
(standardized coefficient was .20 (p < .01) for European Americans and .35 (p < .01) for African 
Americans). There was no effect of 14-month routines on Hispanic children?s social competence. 
 
 
58 
 
In contrast, the effect of 14-month routines on European Americans? and African Americans? 
cognitive skills (see Model 4, Table 17) were approximately the same (standardized coefficients 
were .26 (p < .001) for European Americans and .24 (p < .001) for African Americans), 
suggesting that race/ethnicity likely moderates the link between family routines at 14 months and 
children?s 36-month social competence but not their cognitive skills. Again, there was no effect 
of 14-month routines on Hispanic children?s cognitive ability. Regardless of the size of the 
effect, it is important to note that family routines at 14 months was a consistent positive predictor 
of children?s social competence and cognitive ability for European Americans and African 
Americans, but not Hispanics. A comparison of the model fit statistics between the model 
allowing only the measurement models to be freely estimated (Model 3) and the fully 
unconstrained model (Model 4) indicated that the model in which only the measurement model 
was freely estimated provided the best fit to the data. This suggests that, on average, the link 
between family routines at 14 months and children?s social competence at 36 months is stronger 
for African Americans than for European Americans because routines may be more important 
for African American families, not because the number of routines is higher. 
Family routines at 24 and 36 months. A similar taxonomy of models was fitted for 
family routines at 24 and 36 months and the findings were comparable with two exceptions. 
First, unlike 14-month routines, the effect of 24- and 36-month routines on children?s cognitive 
skills was larger for African Americans than European Americans (i.e., standardized coefficients 
were .22 (p < .001) for European Americans and .34 (p < .001) for African Americans at 24 
months; .22 (p < .001) for European Americans and .26 (p < .001) for African Americans at 36 
months), suggesting that race/ethnicity likely moderates the effect of family routines at 24- and 
36-months on children?s social and cognitive outcomes in both the model allowing the regression 
 
 
59 
 
to be freely estimated and the fully unconstrained model. Second, despite the effects of routines 
being strongest for African Americans, African American families had significantly fewer family 
routines when compared to both European American and Hispanic families at 24- and 36-months 
(Note: The results for the 24-month measurement free model are only for social competence as 
the cognitive ability model failed to identify). In addition, the factor loadings indicate that while 
the observed indicators of routines maintain the same rank order when describing routines for all 
three racial/ethnic groups, they more strongly contribute to European American?s and Hispanic?s 
outcomes. 
  
 
 
60 
 
Discussion 
Thirteen million American children live in families with incomes below the federal 
poverty level, which in 2010 was $22,050 a year for a family of four, and 6.2 million of them 
live in extremely poor families (Wight, Chau, & Aratani, 2011). Official poverty rates are 
highest for young children: 22% of children under age 6 live in poverty. Children in poverty 
experience myriad hardships including food insecurity, crowded housing, and lack of health 
insurance, among others. The hardships associated with poverty have severe negative 
consequences for children?s development including poor cognitive, social, and academic 
outcomes (Duncan et al., 1994), many of which tend to have lasting effects (Duncan et al., 2010). 
Thus, understanding not just the mechanisms by which poverty affects development, but also 
how we can most effectively support positive outcomes across domains is crucial.   
There is evidence to suggest that family routines can be used by parents as a means to 
buffer the effects of challenging contexts for school-aged children (Brody & Flor, 1997; Fiese & 
Everhard, 2008) but we know little about the role of these routines in very young children?s 
development. Importantly, little research has investigated the contribution that routines make to 
children?s development above and beyond that of general parenting. Thus, the purpose of this 
study was to examine the relationship between family routines at 14, 24 and 36 months and 
preschoolers? social competence and cognitive ability at 36 months, above and beyond general 
parenting. Family routines include family-level behaviors (e.g., mealtime) and child activities 
(e.g., homework time) that are supervised or arranged by an adult, all of which provide order, 
predictability, and structure to everyday life (McLoyd et al., 2008). Five separate family routines 
 
 
61 
 
- parent-child play, parent-child outside activities, bedtime routine, regular bedtime, and reading 
routine - were considered in the current study. This allowed for the exploration of the role of 
general family routines as well as activity-specific routines in children?s social and cognitive 
outcomes, providing critical insight into whether certain family routines contribute more to 
positive development than other routines, as well as whether there are differential contributions 
of specific routines to specific outcomes. Additionally, exploring family routines and general 
parenting as two separate constructs allows us to understand the role of family routines beyond 
general parenting. While this has not yet been examined, it is necessary to fully understand the 
benefits of family routines as a prevention or intervention tool. Utilizing longitudinal data 
exploring a young, higher risk sample adds to the current family routines literature and provides 
novel findings that can assist practitioners and teachers working with low-income families as 
well as low-income parents themselves.   
Consistent with prior research which reported that the presence of family routines was 
associated with cooperative, compliant child behavior, social skills, and academic achievement 
among young low-income children (Fiese, 2002; Keltner, 1990; Koblinsky et al., 2006), I found 
that family routines were associated with higher levels of social competence and cognitive ability 
at 36 months. More specifically, children in families that reported higher levels of routines at 14 
and 24 months had higher levels of social competence and cognitive ability at 36 months, 
suggesting there may be both long-term and additive effects of family routines on children?s 
outcomes. These findings are consistent with previous research, which reported that family 
routines at 4-years of age predicted academic achievement at 9-years of age (Fiese, 2002). These 
results suggest that the influences of family routines during early development are not only 
immediate, but are evident months or years later. These findings are similar to research on the 
 
 
62 
 
effects of childhood poverty, which indicates that family income in the first five years of life is 
highly correlated with later developmental outcomes (Duncan et al., 1998; Linver et al., 2002). 
Since preschool development sets the stage for the transition into early childhood, adolescence, 
and even adulthood, family routines must be established early in an effort to reduce the effects of 
the often chaotic environment in which high risk children develop.     
Interestingly, when 36-month family routines were taken into account, findings differed 
for social and cognitive outcomes. More specifically, while early routines do matter for 
children?s later social competence, concurrent (i.e., 36 months) family routines appear to 
outweigh the early effects when all three constructs are considered simultaneously. Conversely, 
compared with concurrent family routines, early family routines appear to matter more for 
children?s 36-month cognitive skills, as evidenced by the fact that the 36-month family routines 
construct did not significantly predict cognitive ability when early routines were taken into 
account. These findings provide some evidence that routines in general may work differently at 
different ages and across different outcomes. More specifically, early family routines provide 
structure (Fiese & Wamboldt, 2000), which may reduce, to some extent, the chaos experienced 
by children living in poverty and facilitate children?s cognitive development. This is likely done 
through the increased positive behavioral adjustment associated with the use of family routines 
(Hofferth & Sandberg, 2001; Sytsma et al., 2001). Children in families who utilize routines are 
better able to self-regulate and manage their behaviors (Evans et al., 2001; 2005) which allows 
for children to attune to important information, and to have more opportunities to engage in 
learning activities. In addition, family routines encourage parent-child relationships (Woods & 
Goldstein, 2003), a key context in which natural learning opportunities arise and are utilized in 
the home. In contrast, the salience of later routines for children?s social competence may reflect 
 
 
63 
 
the fact that children are interacting more with peers at this age (i.e., in preschool or child care) 
and family routines may help them transition into their new social world, a critical 
developmental task during this period (Fiese & Everhard, 2008; Fiese & Wamboldt, 2000). 
Future research should not only consider age-salient routines but also multiple outcomes as they 
may be affected differently.   
Further investigation of the observed indicators of family routines at 36 months suggested 
that family routines as a whole rather than activity-specific routines contribute the most to 
children?s social competence at the same age. This likely has more to do with how the routines 
were measured (i.e., primarily as broad dichotomous variables) in the current study than the 
actual type of routine. More research is needed that gathers detailed information (i.e., frequency 
of routine use and what specific bedtime or play activities are utilized) on each routine to fully 
understand the unique role that activity-specific routines might play in children?s social 
development. With that noted, parent-child outside activities was the most salient predictor of 
children?s social competence. Parent-child outside activities included taking the child to religious 
services, visiting a museum or going on an outing such as shopping, to the park, or on a picnic. 
These activities provide the child with an opportunity to meet other children and adults and gain 
experience with social interactions, as well as to practice positive behavior. These findings align 
with past literature which indicates that when everyday community activities, such as grocery 
shopping, visiting the library, playground or recreational activities, or eating out, are used as 
learning opportunities parents report their children have better communication skills, social 
adaptive capabilities, and socialization (Dunst, Bruder, Trivette, & Hamby, 2006; Dunst et al., 
2000; Dunst et al., 2002). Parent-child play, on the other hand, was the least salient family 
routine in predicting children?s social competence. Despite these findings, past research has 
 
 
64 
 
found parent-child play is related to positive child development (Ginsburg, 2007). More 
specifically, play has been found to have positive effects on children?s brain development 
(Tamis-LeMonda, Shannon, Cabrera, & Lamb, 2004), enhance learning readiness and problem-
solving skills (Fisher, 1992), increase social-emotional learning (Zins, 2004), and build enduring 
parent-child relationships critical for positive child development (Henry, 1990; Smith, 1995; 
Tsao, 2002). The fact that findings from the current study did not illustrate this may reflect the 
fact that parent-child play can be difficult to accurately measure because it can be represented in 
many different forms depending on the family. Even within one family, parent-child play likely 
looks different from day to day and week to week. 
Interestingly, routines as a whole appeared less important for explaining variance in 
cognitive ability than the reading routine variable alone. Previous research has demonstrated that 
parent-preschooler reading is related to language growth, emergent literacy, and reading 
achievement in later development (Bus, van IJzendoorn, & Pellegrini, 1995). A regular reading 
routine insures the facilitation of these activities. Shared book reading between parents and 
young children is positively associated with language development and past research has found 
that children who are read to regularly in early childhood enter school more prepared than their 
peers (Britto, Fuligni, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002; Burgess, Hecht, & Lonigan, 2002; Bus et al., 
1995). This may be especially important for high risk children because they tend to be exposed 
to fewer words as well as less quality verbal content than their more affluent peers (Hart & 
Risley, 1992). Bedtime routines also significantly predicted children?s cognitive ability. Past 
research has found children who participate in a regular bedtime routine, such as taking a bath, 
changing into pajamas, and reading a story, get to bed faster and have more satisfying sleep with 
fewer night wakings (Christodulu & Durand, 2004; Mindell, Telofski, Wiegand, & Kurtz, 2009). 
 
 
65 
 
In turn, adequate sleep is positively associated with increased intellectual ability and academic 
achievement (Buckhalt, El-Sheikh, Keller & Kelly, 2009). Clearly there may be differences in 
the relative salience of specific routines for specific outcomes. Future research should continue 
to consider multiple outcomes as well as activity-specific routines in family routines research. 
Previous research by McLoyd et al. (2008) found that routines may buffer the negative 
effects of everyday life stress for school-aged African-American children; however, the authors 
did not include measures of general parenting in their model and in turn questioned whether the 
effects of routines would remain once more conventional measures of parenting were accounted 
for. In the current study, I explored this question and found that routines, as measured here, no 
longer matter when general parenting (i.e., maternal support, cognitive and language stimulation, 
and mild discipline) is considered. Koblinsky and her colleagues (2006) offer the only other 
study to include measures of both positive parenting and family routines in the same model and 
while the authors did not ask if routines matter above and beyond general parenting, the 
coefficients in their study indicate that family routines predicted child outcomes above and 
beyond positive parenting on several occasions, including predicting children?s total social skills, 
cooperation, assertion, and responsibility.  One possible explanation for my findings is that the 
constructs representing family routines and general parenting were highly collinear. That is, 
families with positive parenting practices are perhaps more likely to engage in routines than are 
families with poorer parenting practices (Jordan, 2003). Jordan (2003) found that high rates of 
parental involvement promoted family routines, and low rates of parental monitoring/supervision 
disrupted family routines. In addition, parenting practices accounted for most of the variance in 
child routines, beyond demographic and child behavior variables, further suggesting that 
parenting practices predict family routines. However, Jordan (2003) suggests that the fact that 
 
 
66 
 
family routines were not significantly predicted by family demographic variables, as parenting 
practices generally are, may indicate that family routines do function beyond parenting practices. 
These findings illustrate a critical need to develop methods of measuring routines and general 
parenting more independently. Again, perhaps a more detailed assessment of routines would be 
of assistance, in this case possibly to help differentiate the two constructs. In addition, Koblinsky 
et al.?s (2006) findings indicate it may also be beneficial to look at more specific aspects of child 
outcomes rather than broad overall social competence and cognitive ability measures. The fact 
that family routines did not moderate the relationship between general parenting and child 
outcomes is also likely due to the high collinearity between constructs.  
Previous research by Churchill and Stoneman (2004) found routines were more salient 
for girls than boys utilizing correlational analyses; however, the authors suggested the 
associations should be further explored using more advanced analytic techniques. I used multi-
group analyses and found that while early routines may be critical for fostering social 
competence and cognitive ability among girls, concurrent family routines may have a stronger 
effect on boys? outcomes. These gender differences may be due to boys? delayed maturation 
when compared to girls. For example, past research has found boys exhibit higher levels of 
externalizing behaviors than girls in preschool, which may indicate less developed self-
regulation and social competence (Chen, 2010). In turn, family routines may be more difficult 
for families with boys to establish early on. Interestingly, the effect of 24-month family routines 
on children?s cognitive ability does not vary for girls and boys. Further research is needed to 
fully understand this pattern of findings. 
The effect of family routines on child outcomes also differed by race/ethnicity with the 
strongest effects being found for African-American children, although the effects for European-
 
 
67 
 
Americans were also modest; there were no effects for Hispanic children. These findings provide 
some support for the hypothesis that routines may be especially important for higher risk 
children. Indeed, research suggests that minority status may heighten the risks associated with 
poverty (Duncan et al., 1994; McLoyd, 1990, 1998). For example minority children are more 
likely to exhibit problem behaviors and aggression (Gross et al., 1999; Leadbeater & Bishop, 
1994) as well as perform less well on cognitive tests than do low-income European-American 
children (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1996). In turn, race/ethnicity and income are often highly 
confounded. High risk children often live in homes that are overcrowded and noisy, and their 
home lives are often hectic, unstructured, and unpredictable (Evans et al., 2005; Evans et al., 
2001). This chaos can disrupt typical functioning across multiple domains. Thus, the structure 
provided by family routines, no matter how minimal, may be especially important for supporting 
positive development among high-risk children. Unfortunately, in most studies, income and 
race/ethnicity are highly confounded so we know little about the unique contribution of each. 
Future work is needed to tease apart these effects.   
With more chaos in the home, the presence of only a few routines can lead to a large 
benefit, as evidenced by the impact of family routines on African-American children in the 
current study. Low-income European-American children, while still at risk, may have other 
structures present in their neighborhoods (i.e., social support) and fewer barriers to those 
structures that make the presence of routines less salient to their social competence and cognitive 
ability. Research indicates, for example, that low-income minorities are less likely to have access 
to primary health care (Newacheck, Hughes, & Stoddard, 1996) and quality infant and toddler 
child care (Becerra & Chi, 1992), among other services that may assist parents and increase 
positive child development. Interestingly, family routines at 14, 24, and 36 months had no effect 
 
 
68 
 
on Hispanic children?s social competence or cognitive ability. The lack of significant findings for 
Hispanic children suggests the measurement of family routines may not be culturally sensitive. 
Future research should explore how families within different cultures define family routines. 
Although the level of family routines at 14 months looked similar across cultures, there 
were clear racial/ethnic differences in the quantity of routines at 24 and 36 months. More 
specifically, African-American families had significantly lower levels of family routine usage 
when compared to European-American and Hispanic families. Interestingly, despite having 
fewer routines, African-American children appear to benefit more from family routines than do 
European-American or Hispanic children. This suggests that as children get older, family 
routines may play a differing role depending on the family?s cultural background. Fiese (2006) 
notes the benefits of studying family routines across varying cultures, including revealing social 
values and roles, and she calls for a more process-orientated examination of routines across 
families of different cultural backgrounds. Currently, we lack empirical literature examining the 
differences and similarities of the definition of routines, routine use, and the effects of routines 
across cultural groups. While the current study contributes to this gap in the literature, continued 
work is needed in this area.  
Limitations and Future Directions      
Although the results from the current study contribute to our knowledge about the role of 
family routines in the development of very young children, especially by exploring differences 
by gender and race ethnicity, there were several limitations of this study. First, because of the 
nature of secondary data analysis, there are limits to the measures and variables available to 
create constructs. For example, it would have been ideal to examine a larger range of family 
routines, including measures representing family mealtime; however, only a select number of 
 
 
69 
 
family routines were represented in the current dataset. Furthermore, three of the five items used 
to represent family routines were dichotomous, allowing only for the examination of the 
presence and absence of a given routine; a continuous variable, on the other hand, would allow 
us to examine questions about how much of a given routine is important. Exploring the 
frequency of routine use by utilizing continuous variables would be useful in attempting to 
address how routine use changes over time. Future research should also consider additional items 
allowing for the effects of multiple routine types to be explored. In addition, it is likely the 
measurement of family routines is not culturally sensitive. Focus groups should be conducted to 
fully understand what routines look like in different cultures.  
Furthermore, while the current study was longitudinal, growth was not found and thus the 
latent constructs were used separately by age to predict child outcomes at 36 months.  The 
aforementioned lack of available variability within items may help explain why no growth was 
found. In addition, as demonstrated by the model fit being best for the unconstrained model, in 
the current study routines looked different across time. More specifically, the underlying 
construct was not represented in the same way at 14, 24, and 36 months. Although it is also 
possible that growth is not present in routines from 14 to 36 months because measuring routines 
at 14 months may be too early in development, it is likely these measurement limitations also 
play a role in explaining why I did not identify growth. While a limitation, this may also 
represent reality. As Churchill and Stoneman (2004) suggest, routines likely change as children 
grow, such that routines appropriate for infants might not be for preschoolers. There is currently 
a lack of longitudinal research exploring family routines. A longitudinal design that includes 
more detailed assessments of developmentally appropriate routines is clearly needed.    
 
 
70 
 
Because the family routines and general parenting constructs were so highly correlated, 
the current study did not allow us to answer whether family routines predict preschoolers? social 
competence and cognitive ability beyond general parenting. When both constructs were in the 
model coefficients unexpectedly became negative. The overlapping of the constructs did not 
permit proper interpretation of the coefficients. Furthermore, the current models may have been 
over-controlled. That is, I controlled for earlier family routines and general parenting when 
investigating later family routines and general parenting. I also controlled for income, gender, 
race/ethnicity, and maternal qualities that might be important in predicting social competence 
and cognitive abilities. Future studies should consider more distinct constructs and fewer control 
variables.   
The intercorrelation of the constructs also made it difficult to tease apart and identify the 
unique contributions of family routines versus general parenting. Measures representing positive 
general parenting, or parenting style more specifically, were very limited in the current study.  
Future research is necessary in order to determine how family routines can be measured 
independently of general parenting. In the future, researchers may be able to represent general 
parenting more as parenting style. Evidence from work by Mize and Petitt (1997) and Mounts 
(2002) supports the idea that parenting style and parenting practices are two separate constructs 
and that parenting practices can compensate for weakness is parenting style. Researchers should 
conduct similar testing with family routines representing parenting practices.  
Finally, although the current study offered a diverse sample with respect to race/ethnicity, 
it lacked families representing various income ranges. The study focused on a low-income Head 
Start sample, allowing for an understanding of how family routines influence low-income 
preschoolers; however, the findings could not be compared to middle or high income 
 
 
71 
 
preschoolers. Replication with a nationally representative sample is necessary to fully understand 
whether routines are especially important for higher risk children. 
Implications 
It is clear that family routines are important for children?s positive social and cognitive 
development. Few researchers have focused on the effect of family routines on preschoolers? 
development and even fewer have focused on prosocial skills (most focus on problem behavior). 
In the current study, however, I have taken a strength based approach by focusing on the 
presence of positive parental attributes rather than simply the absence of negative parenting 
attributes. By investigating family routines in this way, practitioners will be able to build upon 
what low-income parents are already doing in their homes by providing strategies that enhance 
or expand upon their daily activity. In addition, by providing low-income parents with a new 
idea, or a ?do? rather than a ?don?t,? practitioners can encourage positive parent-child 
interactions. Relative to other parenting skills (i.e., discipline) and style (i.e., warmth), family 
routines offer a tangible skill that parents can initiate and maintain with assistance. Efforts to 
develop family routines can be presented in concrete steps which allows for family routines to be 
acquired more easily. Most importantly, as previously mentioned, family routines can be tailored 
to a family?s strengths and needs allowing the family to guide their own change. The current 
findings indicate that family routines are a promising tool that should be a facet of 
comprehensive family prevention and intervention efforts in the future. 
  
 
 
72 
 
References 
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and Profile. Burlington, 
VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.  
Achenbach, T.M., & Rescorla, L.A. (2000). Manual of ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. 
Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. 
Ackerman, B. P., Brown, E. D., & Izard, C. E. (2004). The relations between persistent poverty 
and contextual risk and children?s behavior in elementary school. Development 
Psychology, 40, 367-377. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.367  
Administration on Children and Families (2002). Making a difference in the lives of children and 
families: Impacts of Early head start programs on infants and toddlers and their families. 
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Health and Human Sciences.  
Anthony, L. G., Anthony, B. J., Glanville, D. N., Naiman, D. Q., Waanders, C., & Shaffer, S. 
(2005). The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behavior and preschoolers? 
social competence and behavior problems in the classroom. Infant and Child 
Development, 14, 133-154. doi: 10.1002/icd.385 
Baydar, N., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. (1993). Early warning signs of functional 
illiteracy: Predictors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 64, 815-829. 
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: 
Psychological Corporation. 
Becerra, R. M., & Chi, I. (1992). Child care preferences among low-income minority families. 
International Social Work, 35, 35-47. 
 
 
73 
 
Bennett, L. A., Wolin, S. J., & Reiss, D. (1988). Deliberate family process: A strategy for 
protecting children of alcoholics. British Journal of Addiction, 83, 821-829. 
Benzies, K., & Mychasiuk, R. (2009). Fostering family resiliency: A review of the key protective 
factors. Child and Family Social Work, 14, 103-114. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
2206.2008.00586.x  
Boyce, W. T., Jensen, E. W., James, S. A., & Peacock, J. L. (1983). The Family Routines 
Inventory: Theoretical origins. Social Science Medicine, 17, 193-200. 
Berger, L. M., Paxson, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2009). Income and child development. Children and 
Youth Services Review, 31, 978-989. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2009.04.013  
Bradley, R. H. (1989). HOME measurement of maternal responsiveness. New Directions for 
Child Development, 43, 63-74. 
Bradley, R. H. (1995). Environment and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of 
Parenting (pp. 235-261). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.  
Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1984). The relation of infants? home environments to 
achievement test performance in first grade: A follow-up study. Child Development, 55, 
809-809. 
Bradley, R. H., Chazan-Cohen, R., & Raikes, H. (2009). The impact of Early Head Start on 
school readiness: New looks. Early Education and Development, 20, 883-892.  
Brady-Smith, C., O?Brien, C., Berlin, L., Ware, A., & Fauth, R. C. (2000). Child?Parent 
Interaction Rating Scales for the Three-Bag Assessment (36 months). National Center for 
Children and Families, Teachers College, Columbia University. 
 
 
74 
 
Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., Skuban, E. M., & Horowitz, S. M. (2001). Prevalence of 
social-emotional and behavioral problems in a community sample of 1- and 2-year-old 
children. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 811-819. 
Britto, P. R., Fuligni, A. S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Reading, rhymes, and routines: American 
parents and their young children. In N. Halfon & K. T. McLearn (Eds.), Child rearing in 
America: Challenges facing parents with young children. New York, NY: Cambridge 
University Press.  
Brody, G. H., & Flor, D. L. (1997).  Maternal psychological functioning, family processes, and 
child adjustment in rural, single-parent, African American families. Developmental 
Psychology, 33, 1000-1011.  
Brody, G. H., Flor, D. L., & Gibson, N. M. (1999). Linking maternal efficacy beliefs, 
developmental goals, parenting practices, and child competence in rural single-parent 
African American families. Child Development, 70, 1197-1208.  
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The Future of 
Children: Children and Poverty, 7, 55-71. 
Brooks-Gunn, J., Berlin, L. J., Leventhal, T., & Fuligni, A. (2000). Depending on the kindness of 
strangers: Current national data initiatives and developmental research. Child 
Development, 71, 257-267. 
Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K., & Duncan, G. J. (1996). Ethnic differences in children?s 
intelligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal 
characteristics. Child Development, 67, 396-408 
 
 
75 
 
Bub, K. L., McCartney, K., & Willett, J. B. (2007). Behavior problem trajectories and first-grade 
cognitive ability and achievement skills: A latent growth curve analysis. Journal of 
Educational Psychology, 99, 653-670. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.653  
Buckhalt, J. A., El-Sheikh, M., Keller, P. S., & Kelly, R. J. (2009). Concurrent and longitudinal 
relations between children?s sleep and cognitive functioning: The moderating role of 
parent education. Child Development, 80, 875-892. 
Burgess, S.R., Hecht, S.A., & Lonigan, C.J. (2002). Relations of the Home Literacy 
Environment (HLE) to the Development of Reading-Related Abilities: A One-Year 
Longitudinal Study. Reading Research Quarterly, 37, 408?426. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.37.4.4 
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for 
success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. 
Review of Educational Research, 65, 1-21. 
Caldwell, B. & Bradley, B. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. 
Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.  
Chazen-Cohen, R., Ayoub, C., Pan, B. A., Roggman, L., Raikes, H., McKelvey, L., Whiteside-
Mansell, L., & Hart, A. (2007). It takes time: Impacts of Early Head Start that lead to 
reductions in maternal depression two years later. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28, 151-
170. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20127 
Chen, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in externalizing problems among preschool children: 
Implications for early childhood educators. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 
463-474. 
 
 
76 
 
Christodulu, K. V., & Durand, V. M. (2004). Reducing bedtime disturbance and night waking 
using positive bedtime routines and sleep restriction. Focus on Autisim and Other 
Developmental Disabilities, 19, 130-139. doi: 10.1177/10883576040190030101 
Churchill, S. L., & Stoneman, Z. (2004). Correlates of family routines in Head Start families. 
Early Childhood Research and Practice, 6 (1). Available at http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v6n1. 
Accessed 19 July 2010.  
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (1992). A family process model of economic hardship and 
adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Development, 63, 526-541. 
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in midwestern families: Selected findings 
from the first decade of a prospective, longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and 
Family, 64, 361-373. 
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J., (Eds.). (1997). Consequences of growing up poor. New York: 
Russell Sage Foundation.  
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty, welfare reform, and child 
development. Child Development, 71, 188-196. 
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early child 
development. Child Development, 65, 296-318. 
Duncan, G. J., Yeung, W. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Smith, J. R. (1998). How much does childhood 
poverty affect the life chances of children? American Sociological Review, 63, 406-423. 
Duncan, G. J., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Kalil, A. (2010). Early-childhood poverty and adult 
attainment, behavior, and health. Child Development, 81, 306-325.  
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test ? Revised: Manual. 
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.  
 
 
77 
 
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, 
MN: American Guidance Service. 
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (2001). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: 
American Guidance Service.  
Dunst, C. J., Bruder, M. B., Trivette, C. M., & Hamby, D. W. (2006). Everyday activity settings, 
natural learning environments, and early intervention practices. Journal of Policy and 
Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 3, 3-10.  
Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D., Trivette, C. M., Raab, M., & Bruder, M. B. (2000). Everyday family 
and community life and children?s naturally occurring learning opportunities. Journal of 
Early Intervention, 23, 151-164. doi: 10.1177/10538151000230030501 
Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D., Trivette, C. M., Raab, M., & Bruder, M. B. (2002) Young children?s 
participation in everyday family and community activity. Psychological Reports, 91, 875-
897. 
Economics and Statistics Administration. (2009) Income, poverty, and health insurance 
coverage in the United States: 2008 (Report P60-236). Washington, DC: U.S. 
Government Printing Office.   
Elder, G. H. (1979). Historical change in life patters and personality. In P.B. Baltes & O. G. 
Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Lifespan development and behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 117-159). New York: 
Academic Press. 
Evans, G. W., Gonnella, C., Marcynyszyn, L. A., Gentile, L., & Salpekar, N. (2005). The role of 
chaos in poverty and children?s socioemotional adjustment. Psychological Science, 16, 
560-565. 
 
 
78 
 
Evans, G. W., Saltzman, H., & Cooperman, J. L. (2001). Housing quality and children?s 
socioemotional health. Environment and Behavior, 33, 389-399.  
Fiese, B. H. (2002). Routines of daily living and rituals in family life: A glimpse at stability and 
change during the early child-raising years. Zero to Three, 22, 10-13.  
Fiese, B. H. (2006). Family routines and rituals. New Haven: Yale University Press.  
Fiese, B. H., & Everhart, R. S. (2008). Routines. In Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood 
development (pp. 34-41). Boston: Elsevier Academic Publishing.  
Fiese, B. H. & Kline, C. A. (1993). Development of the Family Ritual Questionnaire: Initial 
reliability and validation studies. Journal of Family Psychology, 6, 1-10.  
Fiese, B. H., Tomcho, T. J., Douglas, M., Josephs, K., Poltrock, S., & Baker, T. (2002). A review 
of 50 years of research on naturally occurring family routines and rituals: Cause for 
celebration? Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 381-390. 
Fiese, B. H., & Wamboldt, F. S. (2000). Family routines, rituals, and asthma management: A 
proposal for family-based strategies to increase treatment adherence. Families, Systems 
and Health, 18, 405-418. 
Fisher, E. P. (1992). The impact of play on development: A meta-analysis. Play Culture, 5, 159. 
Flores, R. L. (2004). The effects of poverty on young children?s ability to organize everyday 
events. Journal of Children and Poverty, 10, 99-118. doi: 
10.1080/1079612042000271558 
Flores, G., Tomany-Korman, S. C., & Olson, L. (2005). Racial and ethnic disparities in health-
related early childhood home routines and safety practices. Archives of Pediatric and 
Adolescent Medicine, 159, 158-165. 
 
 
79 
 
Gagnon, S. G., & Nagle, R. J. (2000). Comparison of the revised and original versions of the 
Bayley Scales of Infant Development. School Psychology International, 21, 293?305. 
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children?s adaptation to negative life events and stressed 
environments. Pediatric Annals, 20, 459-466. 
Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and 
maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics, 119, 182-191. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-
2697 
Gross, D., Sambrook, A., & Fogg, L. (1999). Behavior problems among young children in low-
income urban day care centers. Research in Nursing and Health, 22, 15-25.  
Guidubaldi, J., Cleminshaw, H. K., Perry, J. D., Nastasi, B. K., & Lightel, J. (1986). The role of 
selected family environment factors in children?s post-divorce adjustment. Family 
Relations, 35, 141-151.  
Guo, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Harris, K. M. (1996). Parents? labor force attachment and grade 
retention among urban black children. Sociology of Education, 69, 217-236. 
Guo, G., & Harris, K. M. (2000). The mechanisms mediating the effects of poverty on children?s 
intellectual development. Demography, 37, 431-447. 
Halpern, R. (1990). Poverty and early childhood parenting: Toward a framework for 
intervention. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 6-18. 
Hart, B., Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting 
differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. 
Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096-1105. 
Hawley, D. R., & DeHaan, L. (1996). Toward a definition of family resilience: Integrating life-
span and family perspectives. Family Process, 35, 283-298.    
 
 
80 
 
Henry, M. (1990). More than just play: The significance of mutually directed adult, child 
activity. Early Child Development and Care, 60, 35-51. 
Jackson, A. P., Brooks-Gunn, J., Huang, C., & Glassman, M. (2000). Single mothers in low-
wage jobs: Financial strain, parenting, and preschoolers? outcomes. Child Development, 
71, 1409-1423. 
Jensen, E. W., James, S. A., Boyce, W. T., & Hartnett, S. A. (1983). The Family Routines 
Inventory: Development and validation. Social Science Medicine, 17, 201-211.  
Jordan, S. S. (2003). Further validation of the Child Routines Inventory (CRI): Relationship to 
parenting practices, maternal distress and child externalizing behavior (Doctoral 
dissertation, Louisiana State University). Retrieved from 
http://etd.lsu.edu/docs/available/etd-0612103-213510/unrestricted/Jordan_dis.pdf 
Kaiser, A. P., & Delaney E. M. (1996). The effects of poverty on parenting young children. 
Peabody Journal of Education, 71, 66-85. 
Kaminski, J. W., Perou, R., & Claussen, A. H. (April, 2011). Individual, familial, and 
neighborhood predictors of resilient parenting among low-income mothers. Symposium 
presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, CA.   
Keltner, B. (1990). Family characteristics of preschool social competence among Black children 
in a Head Start program. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 21, 95-108.  
Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry, L. M., & Knight, R. T. (2009). 
Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive 
Neuroscience, 21, 1106-1115. 
Koblinsky, S. A., Kuvalanka, K. A., & Randolph, S. M. (2006). Social skills and behavior 
problems of urban, African American preschoolers: Role of parenting practices, family 
 
 
81 
 
conflict, and maternal depression. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76, 554-563. 
doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.554  
Kotchick, B. A., & Forehand, R. (2002). Putting parenting in perspective: A discussion of the 
contextual factors that shape parenting practices. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 
11, 255-269. 
Korenman, S., Miller, J., & Sjaastad, J. (1995). Long-term poverty and child development in the 
United States: Results from the NLSY. Children and Youth Services Review, 17, 127-
155. 
Koulouglioti, C., Cole, R., & Kitzman, H. (2009). The role of children?s routines of daily living, 
supervision, and maternal fatigue in preschool children?s injury risk. Research in Nursing 
and Health, 32, 517-529. doi: 10.1002/nur.20346  
Kubicek, L. F. (2002). Fresh perspectives on young children and family routines. Zero to Three, 
22, 4-9.  
Land, A., C. & The Foundation for Child Development (2010). 2010 Child Well-Being Index. 
Duke University.  
Linver, M. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Kohen, D. E. (2002). Family processes as pathways from 
income to young children?s development. Developmental Psychology, 38, 719-734. doi: 
10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.719  
Leadbeater, B. J., & Bishop, S. J. (1994) Predictors of behavior problems in preschool children 
of innter-city Afro-American and Puerto Rican adolescent mothers. Child Development, 
65, 638-648. 
 
 
82 
 
Loeber, R., & Farrington, D. P. (2000). Young children who commit crime: Epidemiology, 
developmental origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. 
Development and Psychopathology, 12, 737-762. 
Loukas, A., & Prelow, H. M. (2004). Externalizing and internalizing problems in low-income 
Latino early adolescents: Risk, resource, and protective factors. The Journal of Early 
Adolescence, 24, 250-273. doi: 10.1177/0272431604265675   
Lucyshyn, J. M., Albin, R. W., & Nixon, C. D. (1997). Embedding comprehensive behavioral 
support in family ecology: An experimental, single-case analysis. Journal of Consulting 
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 241-251. 
Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions 
and social policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857-885.  
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical 
evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543-562.  
Markson, S., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Family rituals as a protective factor for children with 
asthma. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 25, 471-479.  
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary Magic: Resilience processes in development. American 
Psychologist, 56, 227-238. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.56.3.227 
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions 
from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 
2, 425-444.  
Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable and 
unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American 
Psychologist, 53, 205-220.  
 
 
83 
 
Maupin, A. N., Brophy-Herb, H. E., Schiffman, R. F., & Bocknek, E. L. (2010). Low-income 
parental profiles of coping, resource adequacy, and public assistant receipt: Links to 
parenting. Family Relations, 59, 180-194. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00594.x  
McGroder, S. (2000). Parenting among low-income, African American single mothers with 
preschool-age children: Patterns, predictors, and developmental correlates. Child 
Development, 71, 752-771.  
McIntire, S. A., & Miller, L. A. (2007). Foundations of psychological testing a practice 
approach. Thousand Oaks California: Sage Publications.  
McLeod, J. D., & Shanahan, M. J. (1996). Trajectories of poverty and children?s mental health. 
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37, 207-220.  
McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: 
Psychological distress, parenting and socioemotional development. Child Development, 
61, 311-346.  
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American 
Psychologist, 53, 185-204. 
McLoyd, V. C., Toyokawa, T., & Kaplan, R. (2008). Work demands, work-family conflict, and 
child adjustment in African American families: The mediating role of family routines. 
Journal of Family Issues, 29, 1247-1267. doi: 10.1177/0192513X08320189 
Menaghan, E. G., & Parcel, T. L. (1991). Determining children?s home environments: The 
impact of maternal characteristics and current occupational and family conditions. 
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 417-431. 
Mindell, J. A., Telofski, L. S., Wiegand, M., & Kurtz, E. S. (2009). A nightly bedtime routine: 
Impact on sleep in young children and maternal mood. Sleep, 32, 599-606. 
 
 
84 
 
Mistry, R. S., Biesanz, J. C., Taylor, L. C., Burchinal, M., & Cox, M. J. (2004). Family income 
and its relation to preschool children?s adjustment for families in the NICHD study of 
early child care. Developmental Psychology, 40, 727-745. 
Mistry, R. S., Vandewater, E. A., Huston, A. C., & McLoyd, V. C. (2002). Economic well-being 
and children?s social adjustment: The role of family process in an ethnically diverse low-
income sample. Child Development, 73, 935-951. 
Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers? social coaching, mother-child relationship style, and 
children?s peer competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312-
332. 
Mounts, N. S. (2002). Parental management of adolescent peer relationships in context: The role 
of parenting style. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 58-69. doi: 10.1037//0893-
3200.16.1.58 
Muthen, B., & Curran, P. (1997). General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in 
experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation. 
Psychological Methods, 2, 371-402. 
Muth?n, L.K., & Muth?n, B. O. (1998-2007). Mplus User?s Guide. Fifth Edition. Los Angeles, 
CA: Muth?n & Muth?n. 
National Research Council (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early 
childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.  
Newacheck, P. W., Hughes, D. C., & Stoddard, J. J. (1996). Children?s access to primary care: 
Differences by race, income, and insurance status. Pediatrics, 97, 26-32. 
 
 
85 
 
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant?
mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child 
Development, 68, 860?879. 
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive 
symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental 
Psychology, 35, 1297?1310. 
Orthner, D. K., Jones-Sanpei, H., & Williamson, S. (2004). The resilience and strengths of low-
income families. Family Relations, 53, 159-167. 
Patterson, J. M. (2002). Integrating family resilience and family stress theory. Journal of 
Marriage and Family, 64, 349-360. 
Patterson, C. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Vaden, N. A. (1990). Income level, gender, ethnicity, and 
household composition as predictors of children?s school-based competence. Child 
Development, 61, 485-494.  
Petterson, S. M., & Albers, A. B. (2001). Effects of poverty and maternal depression and early 
child development. Child Development, 72, 1794-1813.   
Qi, C. H., & Kaiser, A. P. (2003). Behavior problems of preschool children from low-income 
families: Review of the literature. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23, 188-
216. 
Radke-Yarrow, M., & Brown, E. (1993). Resilience and vulnerability in children of multiple-risk 
families. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 581-592.  
Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general 
population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385?401. 
 
 
86 
 
Ross, C.E., Mirowsky, J., & Huber, J. (1983). Dividing work, sharing work, and in-between: 
Marriage patterns and depression. American Sociological Review, 48, 809?823. 
Russell, M., Harris, B., & Gockel, A. (2008). Parenting in poverty: Perspectives of high-risk 
parents. Journal of Children and Poverty, 14, 83-98. doi: 10.1080/10796120701871322 
Scaramella, L., Barnett, M. A., Sohr-Preston, S. L., McGoron, L., & Weiss, J. (April, 2011). 
Resiliency and sensitive parenting: The role of maternal perceptions and cognitive 
competence. Symposium presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, 
Montreal, CA.   
Seaton, E. K., & Taylor, R. D. (2003). Exploring familial processes in urban, low-income 
African American families. Journal of Family Issues, 24, 627-644. doi: 
10.1177/0192513X03252572 
Seccombe, K. (2002). ?Beating the odds? versus ?changing the odds?: Poverty, resilience, and 
family policy. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 384-394.  
Smith, D. (1995). How play influences development at home and school. Journal of Physical 
Education, 66, 19-23. 
Smith, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. (1997). Consequences of living in poverty for young 
children?s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. In G. Duncan, & J. 
Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Consequences of Growing Up Poor. (pp. 132-189). New York: 
Russell Sage.  
Spiker, D., Ferguson, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1993). Enhancing Maternal Interactive Behavior 
and Child Social Competence in Low Birth Weight, Premature Infants. Child 
Development, 64, 754-768.   
 
 
87 
 
Stipek, D. J., & Ryan, R. H. (1997). Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to learn 
but further to go. Developmental Psychology, 33, 711-723.  
Sytsma, S. E., Kelley, M. L., & Wymer, J. H. (2001). Development and initial validation of the 
child routines inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 
241-251. 
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers and 
mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: Contributions to language and cognitive 
development. Child Development, 75, 1806-1820. 
Taylor, R. D. (1996). Adolescents? perceptions of kinship support and family management 
practices: Association with adolescent adjustment in African American families. Child 
Development, 32, 687-695.  
Takeuchi, D. T., Williams, D. R., & Adair, R. K. (1991). Economic stress in the family and 
children?s emotional and behavioral problems. Journal of Marriage and Family, 53, 
1031-1041. 
Trentacosta, C. J., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2008). 
The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early 
childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1211-1219. doi: 
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01941.x  
Tsao, L. (2002). How much do we know about the importance of play in child development? 
Child Education, 78, 230-233.  
Webster-Stratton, C., & Hammond, M. (1998). Conduct problems and level of social competence 
in Head Start children: Prevalence, pervasiveness, and associated risk factors. Clinical 
Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 101-124.  
 
 
88 
 
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ? Third Edition. 
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.  
Wight, V. R., Chau, M., Aratani, Y. (2011). Who are American?s poor children? : The official 
story. National Center for Children in Poverty, Columbia University Mailman School of 
Public Health.  
Willet, J. B., & Bub, K. L. (2004). Latent growth curve analysis. In D. Rindskoph (Ed.), 
Encyclopedia of statistics in the behavioral sciences. Sussex, UK: John Wiley and Sons.  
Willet, J. B. & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and 
predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363-381.  
Woods, J. & Goldstein, H. (2003). When the toddler takes over: Changing challenging routines 
into conduits for communication. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental 
Disabilities, 18, 176-181. 
Yoos, H. L., Kitzman, H., & Cole, R. (1999). Family routines and the feeding process. In D. B. 
Kessler & P. Dawson (Eds.), Failure to thrive and pediatric undernutrition: A 
transdisciplinary approach (pp. 375-384). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing 
Co.  
Yeung, W. J., Linver, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). How money matters for young 
children?s development: Parental investment and family processes. Child Development, 
73, 1861-1879. 
Zins, J. E. (2004). Building academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the 
research say? New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
 
 
89 
 
Appendices 
 
 
90 
 
Appendix A 
Family Routine Items 
 
 
 
91 
 
Appendix B 
Tables 
Table 1.  
Descriptive Statistics for All Study Variables 
 Age at Assessment 
 14-months 24-months 36-months 
 
Variable 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Outcomes    
BRS: Emotion Regulation   4.01 (.68) 
BRS: Orientation/Engagement   3.93 (.67) 
Child Behavior Checklist 
(Aggressive behavior subscale)   
11.21 
(6.18) 
Mental Development Index   91.33 (12.40) 
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test   83.85 (14.70) 
Control    
Program Group 
(Early Head Start Program Status) 53%   
Male 49%   
  White 48%   
   Black 39%   
   Hispanic 13%   
High School Diploma or GED 67%   
Not Married or Cohabitating 79%   
Maternal Depression 23%   
Income-To-Needs 60.26 (54.21)   
Inadequate Money 19%   
Family Routines    
Bedtime Routine 69% 69% 69% 
Regular Bedtime 62% 58% 57% 
Reading Routines 51% 58% 57% 
Parent-Child Play 4.54 (.76) 4.56 (.81) 4.44 (.80) 
Parent-Child Outside Activity 2.62 (.63) 2.81 (.72) 2.89 (.69) 
General Positive Parenting    
Discipline Severity Index 2.27 (1.53) 2.77 (1.70) 3.52 (1.62) 
   Physical Discipline 17% 31% 51% 
   Prevent/Distract 62% 70% 67% 
 
 
92 
 
   Threaten 31% 34% 11% 
   Shout 3% 5% 8% 
   Talk/Explain 20% 34% 69% 
   Remove Object 78% 82% 82% 
   Time Out - - 31% 
   Warn/Remind Nonverbal - - 33% 
   Warn/Remind Verbal - - 8% 
   Restrain - - 25% 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation 
10.12 
(1.53) 
10.54 
(1.36) 
10.79 
(1.86) 
Maternal Supportiveness 4.04 (1.03) 4.05 (1.04) 4.00 (.91) 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
93 
 
Table 2.  
Descriptive Statistics and Individual t-tests for All Study Variables by Gender 
 Male Female t 
 
Variable 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
 
Outcomes    
BRS: Emotion Regulation ? 36m 3.91 (.70) 4.11 (.65) -4.97*** 
BRS: Orientation/Engagement ? 
36m 
3.89 
(.67) 
3.97 
(.66) -2.17
* 
Child Behavior Checklist ? 36m 
(Aggressive behavior subscale) 
11.69 
(6.50) 
10.75 
(5.83) 2.67
** 
Mental Development Index ? 36m 89.97 (12.49) 92.62 (12.18) -3.76*** 
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 
? 36m 
82.48 
(14.59) 
85.15 
(15.70) -3.19
*** 
Family Routines    
Bedtime Routine ? 14m 67% 72% -1.74+ 
Bedtime Routine ? 24m 69% 69% -.01 
Bedtime Routine ? 36m 67% 71% -1.39 
Regular Bedtime ? 14m 62% 61% .45 
Regular Bedtime ? 24m 57% 59% -.94 
Regular Bedtime ? 36m 58% 57% .35 
Reading Routines ? 14m 47% 56% -2.96** 
Reading Routines ? 24m 55% 60% -1.83+ 
Reading Routines ? 36m 54% 60% -2.08* 
Parent-Child Play ? 14m 4.50 (.77) 4.58 (.75) -1.59 
Parent-Child Play ? 24m 4.53 (.82) 4.60 (.80) -1.54 
Parent-Child Play ? 36m 4.37 (.82) 4.50 (.78) -2.59** 
Parent-Child Outside Activity ? 
14m 
2.60 
(.61) 
2.63 
(.65) -.99 
Parent-Child Outside Activity ? 
24m 
2.79 
(.68) 
2.82 
(.75) -.76 
Parent-Child Outside Activity ? 
36m 
2.89 
(.69) 
2.90 
(.68) -.24 
General Positive Parenting    
Discipline Severity Index ? 14m 2.34 (1.58) 2.20 (.90) 1.54 
Discipline Severity Index ? 24m 2.80 (1.69) 2.75 (1.69) .42 
Discipline Severity Index ? 36m 3.53 (1.61) 3.50 (1.62) .26 
   Physical Discipline ? 14m 20% 15% 2.23*** 
   Physical Discipline ? 24m 31% 30% .38 
   Physical Discipline ? 36m 52% 51% .26 
   Prevent/Distract ? 14m 60% 65% -1.85+ 
 
 
94 
 
 
 
 
  
   Prevent/Distract ? 24m 68% 72% -1.34 
   Prevent/Distract ? 36m 67% 67% .15 
   Threaten ? 14m 30% 32% -.64 
   Threaten ? 24m 35% 34% .57 
   Threaten ? 36m 11% 11% -.29 
   Shout ? 14m 3% 3% -.70 
   Shout ? 24m 4% 5% -.50 
   Shout ? 36m 9% 7% 1.14 
   Talk/Explain -14m 19% 21% -1.07 
   Talk/Explain -24m 32% 36% -1.34 
   Talk/Explain -36m 68% 69% -.45 
   Remove Object ? 14m 78% 78% -.17 
   Remove Object ? 24m 83% 82% .58 
   Remove Object ? 36m 81% 83% -.70 
   Time Out ? 36m 30% 31% -.49 
   Warn/Remind Nonverbal ? 36m 33% 34% -.54 
   Warn/Remind Verbal ? 36m 9% 7% 1.13 
   Restrain ? 36m 26% 24% .72 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 14m 
10.05 
(1.55) 
10.18 
(1.52) -1.38 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 24m 
10.44 
(1.49) 
10.62 
(1.23) -2.11
* 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 36m 
10.62 
(1.94) 
10.95 
(1.78) -3.11
** 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 14m 3.97 (1.06) 4.11 (.99) -2.23* 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 24m 3.99 (1.09) 4.11 (.99) -1.84+ 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 36m 3.97 (.93) 4.03 (.90) -1.22 
***p  < .001  **p  < .01   *p  < .05   +p  < .10   
 
 
 
95 
 
Table 3. 
Descriptive Statistics and Individual ANOVAS for All Study Variables by Race/Ethnicity 
 White Black Hispanic F 
 
Variable 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
Mean/%  
(SD) 
 
Outcomes     
BRS: Emotion Regulation 
? 36m 
4.02 
(.68) 
4.02 
(.67) 
3.98 
(.73) .66 
BRS: 
Orientation/Engagement ? 
36m 
4.05 
(.62) 
3.78 
(.68) 
3.92 
(.70) 14.08*** 
Child Behavior Checklist 
? 36m 
(Aggressive behavior 
subscale) 
11.66 
(6.15) 
10.91 
(6.15) 
10.94 
(6.47) 2.78* 
Mental Development 
Index ? 36m 
94.92 
(12.18) 
87.51 
(11.57) 
90.29 
(11.69) 32.69
*** 
Peabody Picture 
Vocabulary Test ? 36m 
88.45 
(13.86) 
80.43 
(12.92) 
77.32 
(17.64) 40.03
*** 
Family Routines     
Bedtime Routine ? 14m 75% 63% 67% 5.59*** 
Bedtime Routine ? 24m 75% 60% 71% 7.89*** 
Bedtime Routine ? 36m 74% 63% 67% 5.46*** 
Regular Bedtime ? 14m 69% 54% 56% 7.77*** 
Regular Bedtime ? 24m 63% 53% 55% 3.63* 
Regular Bedtime ? 36m 62% 52% 52% 5.59*** 
Reading Routines ? 14m 55% 50% 46% 2.22+ 
Reading Routines ? 24m 66% 49% 54% 8.27*** 
Reading Routines ? 36m 66% 47% 55% 13.08*** 
Parent-Child Play ? 14m 4.57 (.74) 4.53 (.80) 4.51 (.76) .77 
Parent-Child Play ? 24m 4.61 (.78) 4.48 (.85) 4.64 (.74) 2.50+ 
Parent-Child Play ? 36m 4.50 (.78) 4.37 (.80) 4.37 (.87) 2.95* 
Parent-Child Outside 
Activity ? 14m 
2.52 
(.53) 
2.68 
(.68) 
2.80 
(.70) 9.33
*** 
Parent-Child Outside 
Activity ? 24m 
2.67 
(.63) 
2.92 
(.82) 
2.95 
(.68) 10.97
*** 
Parent-Child Outside 
Activity ? 36m 
2.80 
(.61) 
2.99 
(.78) 
2.86 
(.61) 8.20
*** 
General Positive 
Parenting   
  
Discipline Severity Index 
? 14m 
1.98 
(1.33) 
2.79 
(1.67) 
1.87 
(1.35) 28.07
*** 
Discipline Severity Index 
? 24m 
2.34 
(1.56) 
3.58 
(1.68) 
2.19 
(1.34) 53.13
*** 
Discipline Severity Index 
? 36m 
2.91 
(1.57) 
4.41 
(1.24) 
3.22 
(1.58) 93.35
*** 
   Physical Discipline ? 
14m 10% 29% 
10% 24.51*** 
   Physical Discipline ? 
24m 19% 54% 
11% 59.42*** 
 
 
96 
 
 
  
   Physical Discipline ? 
36m 32% 80% 
41% 101.53*** 
   Prevent/Distract ? 14m 63% 55% 77% 8.27*** 
   Prevent/Distract ? 24m 70% 66% 81% 3.93* 
   Prevent/Distract ? 36m 69% 61% 76% 5.11** 
   Threaten ? 14m 30% 36% 22% 3.41* 
   Threaten ? 24m 30% 42% 31% 5.73*** 
   Threaten ? 36m 8% 12% 17% 3.95** 
   Shout -14m 2% 4% 2% 1.74 
   Shout -24m 4% 5% 6% .16 
   Shout -36m 7% 9% 8% .79 
   Talk/Explain -14m 23% 17% 18% 1.79 
   Talk/Explain -24m 35% 32% 38% .59 
   Talk/Explain -36m 70% 64% 76% 3.86** 
   Remove Object ? 14m 81% 75% 76% 1.73 
   Remove Object ? 24m 86% 79% 77% 3.29* 
   Remove Object ? 36m 90% 73% 81% 17.77*** 
   Time Out ? 36m 40% 22% 24% 14.92*** 
   Warn/Remind 
Nonverbal ? 36m 29% 40% 
29% 5.90*** 
   Warn/Remind Verbal ? 
36m 7% 8% 
13% 2.20+ 
   Restrain ? 36m 28% 21% 25% 2.03 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 14m 
10.54 
(1.29) 
9.78 
(1.66) 
9.73 
(1.57) 25.00
*** 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 24m 
10.91 
(1.07) 
10.14 
(1.52) 
10.34 
(1.43) 26.82
*** 
Language and Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 36m 
11.18 
(1.73) 
10.31 
(1.98) 
10.73 
(1.72) 18.56
*** 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 
14m 
4.38 
(.98) 
3.64 
(1.01) 
3.93 
(.89) 39.76
*** 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 
24m 
4.32 
(1.00) 
3.64 
(1.03) 
4.10 
(.91) 31.11
*** 
Maternal Supportiveness ? 
36m 
4.15 
(.88) 
3.80 
(.94) 
4.06 
(.88) 12.08
*** 
***p  < .001  **p  < .01   *p  < .05   +p  < .10   
 
 
 
97 
 
  
  
***
p  
< .001
  **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 Table
 4 
Correl
ations
 for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 14
-month
s 
 
 
1.
 
2.
 
3.
 
4.
 
5.
 
6.
 
7.
 
8.
 
9.
 
10.
 
11.
 
12.
 
1
.
 
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
36m
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
-
36m
 
.
2
9
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3
.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
Ag
g
r
ess
io
n
 
Su
b
s
ca
le
 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
-
.0
5
+
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4
.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t I
n
d
e
x
 
?
 
36m
 
.
32
*
*
*
 
.
37
**
*
 
-
.
08
*
*
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
-
36m
 
.
2
7
***
 
.
3
7
**
*
 
-
.
09
*
*
 
.
5
7
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
5
+
 
.0
3
 
.0
5
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
. 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
-
.
06
*
 
.
0
8
*
*
 
-
.
11
***
 
-
.0
9
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
. 
W
h
ite
 
.
0
2
 
.
1
8
***
 
.
0
6
*
 
.
2
7
***
 
.
3
0
***
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
4
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
9
. 
B
lack
 
.
0
0
0
 
-
.
1
8
**
*
 
-
.
0
5
 
-
.
2
5
***
 
-
.
1
8
***
 
.
0
2
 
.
0
3
 
-
.
7
6
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
1
0
. 
His
p
an
ic
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
1
7
***
 
.
0
2
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
3
8
***
 
-
.
3
1
***
 
--
 
 
 
11
. 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
.
0
4
 
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
1
 
.
2
2
***
 
.
1
9
***
 
.
0
0
3
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
1
8
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
--
 
 
12
. 
No
t 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
-
.
1
0
**
*
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
4
 
-
.
1
6
***
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
.
0
5
 
.
0
3
 
-
.
3
3
***
 
.
2
9
***
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
--
 
13
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
8
+
 
.
1
2
**
 
-
.
1
1
**
 
-
.
1
2
**
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
0
8
+
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
0
4
 
1
4
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
3
 
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
3
 
.
0
0
4
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
.
0
5
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.
2
4
***
 
1
5
.
 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
0
3
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
5
**
 
.
0
0
4
 
1
6
.
 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
0
5
+
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
1
1
***
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.
0
0
2
 
-
.
0
5
+
 
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
1
7
.
 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
8
**
 
-
.
0
6
*
 
.
1
1
***
 
.
1
2
***
 
.
0
2
 
.
0
1
 
.
1
4
***
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
1
0
**
 
-
.
0
5
+
 
1
8
.
 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
.
0
4
 
.
0
8
**
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
.
1
0
***
 
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
5
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
2
9
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
9
**
 
-
.
0
9
***
 
.
0
9
**
 
.
0
8
**
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
5
*
 
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
0
0
3
 
-
.
0
3
 
2
0
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
.
0
8
**
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
1
5
***
 
.
0
8
*
 
.
1
1
***
 
.
0
3
 
-
.
0
3
 
2
1
.
 
Dis
cip
lin
e
 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
-
.
0
0
2
 
-
.
1
0
***
 
.
0
5
+
 
-
.
1
3
***
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
0
5
 
-
.
1
9
***
 
.
2
7
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
-
.
0
6
+
 
.
1
2
***
 
2
2
.
 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
.
0
8
*
 
.
1
8
***
 
-
.
0
8
**
 
.
2
3
***
 
.
2
8
***
 
.
0
0
2
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
2
6
***
 
-
.
1
9
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
.
1
9
***
 
-
.
1
7
***
 
23
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
.
1
5
**
*
 
.
2
0
***
 
-
.
0
8
*
 
.
3
7
***
 
.
35
*
**
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
.
3
2
***
 
-
.
3
0
***
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
2
0
***
 
-
.
2
2
***
 
 
 
98 
 
 
  
Table
 4 (c
on
t.) 
Correl
ations
 for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 14
-month
s 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01
   *
p  
< .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
13.
 
14.
 
15.
 
16.
 
17.
 
18.
 
19.
 
20.
 
21.
 
22.
 
23.
 
1
.
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3
.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4
.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t I
n
d
e
x
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
.
 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
.
 
W
h
ite
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
9
.
 
B
lack
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
0
.
 
His
p
an
ic
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
1
.
 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
2
.
 
No
t 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
3
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
4
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
-
.
0
7
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
5
.
 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
.
1
3
**
 
-
.1
0
**
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
6
.
 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
3
 
.
0
2
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
7
.
 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
-
.
1
2
**
 
.
0
9
**
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
2
5
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
8
.
 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
-
.
0
7
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
1
6
***
 
.
1
5
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
9
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
0
0
4
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
2
0
***
 
.
1
7
***
 
.
5
1
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
2
0
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
5
 
-
.
0
2
 
.
1
2
***
 
.
0
2
 
.
2
4
***
 
.
3
4
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
21
. 
Dis
cip
lin
e
 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
.
0
6
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
-
.
1
3
***
 
-
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
5
+
 
.
0
6
*
 
--
 
 
 
2
2
.
 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
-
.
1
5
***
 
.1
0
**
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
1
9
***
 
.
2
1
***
 
.
3
3
***
 
.
3
5
***
 
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
--
 
 
2
3
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
0
5
 
.
0
5
 
.
1
0
**
 
.
1
0
***
 
.
1
0
***
 
.
1
3
***
 
.0
4
 
-
.
1
8
***
 
.
2
9
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
99 
 
 
  
Table
 5 
Correl
atio
ns for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 24
-month
s 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
1.
 
2.
 
3.
 
4.
 
5.
 
6.
 
7.
 
8.
 
9.
 
10.
 
11.
 
12.
 
1.
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
?
 
36m
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
?
 
36m
 
.
29
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
Ag
g
r
ess
io
n
 
Su
b
s
ca
le
 
?
 
36m
 
-
.1
4
***
 
-
.0
5
+
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t I
n
d
e
x
 
?
 
36m
 
.
3
2
*
*
*
 
.
3
7
**
*
 
-
.
08
*
*
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
-
36m
 
.
27
***
 
.
37
**
*
 
-
.
09
*
*
 
.
5
7
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
1
 
-
.0
5
+
 
.0
3
 
.0
5
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
. 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
-
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
8
*
*
 
-
.
11
***
 
-
.0
9
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
. 
W
h
ite
 
.0
2
 
.
18
***
 
.0
6
*
 
.2
7
***
 
.
30
***
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
4
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
9
. 
B
lack
 
.0
0
 
-
.
18
**
 
-
.0
5
 
-
.
25
***
 
-
.1
8
***
 
.0
2
 
.
0
3
 
-
.
76
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
1
0
. 
His
p
an
ic
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.
01
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.1
7
***
 
.0
2
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
38
***
 
-
.
31
***
 
--
 
 
 
11
. 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
.0
4
 
.
09
**
 
.0
1
 
.
22
***
 
.1
9
***
 
.0
03
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
18
***
 
-
.
1
1
 
-
.
11
***
 
--
 
 
12
.
 
No
t
 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
-
.
10
**
*
 
-
.0
9
**
 
.0
4
 
-
.1
6
***
 
-
.1
2
***
 
.0
5
 
.0
3
 
-
.
33
***
 
.
29
***
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.0
9
**
 
--
 
13
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.
08
+
 
.1
2
**
 
-
.1
1
**
 
-
.1
2
**
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.0
5
 
.
08
+
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.0
3
 
.0
4
 
1
4
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
.0
6
*
 
.0
2
 
-
.0
9
**
 
.0
3
 
.
09
**
 
.0
3
 
.0
04
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.1
1
***
 
.0
5
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.2
4
***
 
1
5
.
 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
1
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.0
3
 
.
0
3
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.0
5
 
.0
04
 
1
6
.
 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
8
**
 
-
.0
5
 
.1
5
***
 
.
1
7
***
 
.0
3
 
.0
0
 
.
1
3
***
 
-
.1
5
***
 
.
0
2
 
.0
8
*
 
-
.0
4
 
1
7
.
 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
.0
2
 
.0
3
 
-
.0
6
*
 
.0
6
+
 
.
0
4
 
.0
3
 
-
.0
3
 
.1
0
***
 
-
.0
8
**
 
-
.0
3
 
.
10
**
 
-
.0
4
 
1
8
.
 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
.
0
4
 
.1
1
***
 
-
.
10
***
 
.
1
8
***
 
.1
8
***
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
6
+
 
.1
5
***
 
-
.
13
***
 
-
.
03
 
.
06
+
 
-
.
07
 
1
9
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
-
.0
1
 
.
1
5
***
 
-
.
1
0
***
 
.1
4
***
 
.
1
1
***
 
.0
4
 
-
.0
5
 
.0
5
+
 
-
.
08
**
 
.0
4
 
.0
2
 
-
.
0
1
 
2
0
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
-
.0
04
 
.0
2
 
-
.1
2
***
 
.0
2
 
-
.0
1
 
.0
1
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.1
8
***
 
.
1
3
***
 
.
0
8
*
 
-
.0
3
 
.
0
2
 
2
1
.
 
Dis
cip
lin
e
 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
1
0
***
 
-
.
003
 
-
.
1
7
***
 
.1
3
***
 
-
.
0
8
**
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
2
6
***
 
.
3
6
***
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
.
1
7
***
 
2
2
.
 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
.
0
8
*
 
.
14
***
 
-
.0
6
*
 
.
30
***
 
.
2
8
***
 
.0
4
 
-
.0
6
*
 
.
27
***
 
-
.
23
***
 
-
.
06
+
 
.
22
***
 
-
.
0
1
***
 
23
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
.
10
*
*
 
.
1
3
***
 
-
.
09
**
 
.
3
5
***
 
.
3
1
*
**
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.0
6
+
 
.2
7
***
 
-
.2
9
***
 
.0
2
 
.
20
***
 
-
.1
5
***
 
 
 
100 
 
 
  
Table
 5 (c
on
t.) 
Correl
atio
ns for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 24
-month
s 
***
p  
< .001
  **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
13.
 
14.
 
15.
 
16.
 
17.
 
18.
 
19.
 
20.
 
21.
 
22.
 
23.
 
1
.
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3
.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4
.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t 
I
n
d
e
x
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
.
 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
.
 
W
h
ite
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
9
.
 
B
lack
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
0
.
 
His
p
an
ic
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
1
.
 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
12.
 
No
t
 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
3
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
4
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
-
.0
7
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
5
.
 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
.
1
3
**
 
-
.1
0
**
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
6
.
 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
-
.1
2
*
 
.0
02
 
.
0
1
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
7
.
 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
.
0
1
 
.0
2
 
.
0
7
*
 
.2
6
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
8
.
 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
-
.
0
6
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.0
3
 
.1
2
***
 
.1
2
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
9
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
0
1
 
.
0
0
4
 
.1
2
***
 
.
1
7
***
 
.
6
1
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
2
0
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
.0
3
 
-
.0
04
 
-
.0
1
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
0
1
 
.2
1
***
 
.
3
6
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
2
1
.
 
Dis
cip
lin
e
 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
.
0
8
+
 
-
.
0
5
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
0
9
**
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.
1
3
***
 
-
.
0
5
 
.
0
9
**
 
--
 
 
 
2
2
.
 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
-
.
1
6
***
 
.
10
**
 
-
.
0
7
*
 
.1
6
***
 
.1
4
***
 
.3
2
***
 
.3
5
***
 
.
1
2
***
 
-
.
2
0
***
 
--
 
 
2
3
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
-
.0
8
+
 
.1
5
***
 
.
0
2
 
.1
5
***
 
.1
1
***
 
.
2
1
***
 
.
1
7
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.
2
1
***
 
.2
6
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
101 
 
 
  
Table
 6 
Correl
atio
ns for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 36
-month
s 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
1.
 
2.
 
3.
 
4.
 
5.
 
6.
 
7.
 
8.
 
9.
 
10.
 
11.
 
12.
 
1.
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
-
36m
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
-
36m
 
.
29
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3
.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
Ag
g
r
ess
io
n
 
Su
b
s
ca
le
 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.1
4
***
 
-
.0
5
+
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4
.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t I
n
d
e
x
 
-
3
6
m
 
.
32
*
*
*
 
.
3
7
**
*
 
-
.
08
*
*
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
-
36m
 
.
27
***
 
.
.
3
7
**
*
 
-
.
09
*
*
 
.
5
7
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
0
1
 
-
.0
5
+
 
.0
3
 
.0
5
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
. 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
-
.
1
4
***
 
-
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
8
*
*
 
-
.
11
***
 
-
.0
9
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
. 
W
h
ite
 
.0
2
 
.
18
***
 
.0
6
*
 
.2
7
***
 
.
30
***
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
4
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
9
. 
B
lack
 
.0
0
 
-
.
18
**
 
-
.0
5
 
-
.
25
***
 
-
.1
8
***
 
.0
2
 
.0
3
 
-
.
76
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
10
. 
His
p
an
ic
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.
01
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.1
7
***
 
.0
2
 
.
0
2
 
-
.
38
***
 
-
.
31
***
 
--
 
 
 
11
. 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
.0
4
 
.
09
**
 
.
0
1
 
.
22
***
 
.1
9
***
 
.0
03
 
-
.
0
3
 
.
18
***
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
-
.
11
***
 
--
 
 
12
. 
No
t 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
-
.
10
***
 
-
.0
9
**
 
.0
4
 
-
.1
6
***
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
.0
5
 
.0
3
 
-
.
33
***
 
.
29
*
**
 
.
0
7
*
 
-
.0
9
**
 
--
 
13
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.
08
+
 
.1
2
**
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
-
.1
2
**
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.0
5
 
.
08
+
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.0
3
 
.0
4
 
14
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
.0
6
*
 
.0
2
 
-
.0
9
**
 
.0
3
 
.
09
**
 
.0
3
 
.0
04
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.1
1
***
 
.0
5
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.2
4
***
 
15
. 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
2
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
3
 
-
.0
2
 
-
.0
3
 
.0
3
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
4
 
-
.
0
4
 
-
.0
5
 
.0
04
 
16
. 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
.
0
6
*
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
11
***
 
.
1
0
***
 
.
08
**
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.0
4
 
.1
0
***
 
-
.
10
***
 
-
.0
2
 
.0
1
 
-
.0
7
*
*
 
17
. 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
.
0
6
*
 
.0
5
 
-
.1
3
***
 
.0
8
**
 
.
0
5
+
 
.
0
2
 
-
.0
1
 
.
10
***
 
-
.0
8
**
 
-
.
0
4
 
.0
9
**
 
-
.0
6
*
 
18
. 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
.0
5
 
.1
3
***
 
-
.1
4
***
 
.
19
***
 
.1
6
***
 
.
0
5
+
 
-
.0
6
*
 
.1
7
***
 
-
.
17
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
.
05
 
-
.0
7
*
 
19
. 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
.0
2
 
.1
4
***
 
-
.1
3
***
 
.
10
***
 
.
1
0
***
 
.0
6
*
 
-
.0
7
**
 
.
08
**
 
-
.
0
6
+
 
-
.
03
 
-
.0
1
 
.
00
 
20
. 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
.
0
7
*
 
.0
8
**
 
-
.1
4
***
 
-
.0
4
 
.0
2
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
.1
3
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.0
4
 
.
0
0
4
 
2
1
.
 
Dis
cip
lin
e 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
-
.
0
3
 
-
.
1
6
***
 
.
1
3
***
 
-
.
2
1
***
 
-
.
2
2
***
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
0
1
 
-
.
3
7
***
 
.
4
3
***
 
-
.
0
8
**
 
-
.
0
9
*
 
.
2
0
***
 
22
. 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
.1
1
***
 
.
28
***
 
-
.1
4
***
 
.3
7
***
 
.3
6
***
 
.0
5
+
 
-
.0
9
**
 
.2
0
***
 
-
.
20
***
 
-
.
01
 
.2
2
***
 
-
.
17
***
 
23
. 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
.
1
4
**
*
 
.
1
2
***
 
-
.0
7
*
 
.
2
8
***
 
.
2
3
*
**
 
.
0
6
*
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
15
***
 
-
.1
8
***
 
.
0
2
 
.2
1
***
 
-
.
11
***
 
 
 
102 
 
 
  
Table
 6 (c
on
t.) 
Correl
atio
ns for Al
l S
tud
y Vari
ables f
or 36
-month
s 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
1
3
.
 
14
.
 
15
.
 
16
.
 
17
.
 
18
.
 
19
.
 
20
.
 
21
.
 
22
.
 
23
.
 
1
.
 
B
R
S: E
m
o
tio
n
 
R
e
g
u
latio
n
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
2
.
 
B
R
S: Or
ien
tatio
n
/En
g
ag
e
m
en
t
 
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
3
.
 
C
h
ild
 
B
eh
av
io
r
 
C
h
ec
k
lis
t
 
 
.
6
7
*
*
*
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
4
.
 
Me
n
tal 
Dev
elo
p
m
e
n
t I
n
d
e
x
 
 
.
4
6
*
*
*
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
5
.
 
P
ea
b
o
d
y
 
P
ictu
r
e 
Vo
ca
b
u
lar
y
 
T
est
 
 
-
.
0
4
*
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
6
.
 
Pro
g
r
am
 
Gr
o
u
p
 
 
.
0
8
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
7
. 
C
h
ild
 
Sex
 
 
-
.
1
3
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
8
. 
W
h
ite
 
 
-
.
1
2
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
9
. 
B
lack
 
 
.
0
1
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
10
. 
His
p
an
ic
 
 
-
.
0
6
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
11
. 
E
d
u
ca
tio
n
 
L
ev
el
 
 
.
0
6
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
12
. 
No
t 
Ma
r
r
ied
 
o
r
 
C
o
h
ab
itati
n
g
 
 
-
.
1
8
***
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
13
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Dep
r
ess
io
n
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
14
.
 
I
n
co
m
e
-
to
-
Nee
d
s
 
-
.0
7
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
5
.
 
I
n
ad
eq
u
ate 
Mo
n
ey
 
.
1
3
**
 
-
.1
0
**
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
6
.
 
B
e
d
tim
e 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
-
.0
4
 
.0
5
 
.
0
1
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
7
.
 
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
ti
m
e
 
-
.
0
5
 
.0
4
 
.0
4
 
.2
4
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
8
.
 
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
tin
e
 
-
.1
2
**
 
.
10
**
 
-
.
0
0
1
 
.1
5
***
 
.1
5
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
9
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
la
y
 
 
-
.0
1
 
.
0
1
 
-
.0
1
 
.1
0
***
 
.
09
**
 
.5
4
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
 
2
0
.
 
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
O
u
ts
id
e 
A
cti
v
it
y
 
.0
04
 
.0
7
*
 
-
.
0
3
 
.0
5
+
 
.
0
3
 
.
19
***
 
.3
7
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
2
1
.
 
Dis
cip
lin
e 
Sev
er
it
y
 
I
n
d
e
x
 
.
0
6
 
-
.
1
1
***
 
-
.
0
1
 
-
.
0
9
***
 
-
.1
4
***
 
-
.1
9
***
 
-
.0
9
**
 
-
.
0
3
 
--
 
 
 
2
2
.
 
L
an
g
u
a
g
e 
an
d
 
C
o
g
n
it
iv
e 
Sti
m
u
lat
io
n
 
-
.
0
4
 
.
10
**
 
-
.0
4
 
.1
7
***
 
.
12
***
 
.
32
***
 
.3
1
***
 
.1
9
***
 
-
.
2
6
***
 
--
 
 
2
3
.
 
Ma
ter
n
al 
Su
p
p
o
r
tiv
en
es
s
 
-
.0
02
 
.0
8
*
 
.
0
4
 
.1
1
***
 
.
0
7
*
 
.1
5
***
 
.
10
***
 
.0
8
*
 
-
.
2
2
***
 
.3
5
***
 
--
 
 
 
 
103 
 
Table 7.  
Stability of Predictors Over Time 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
Variable 14 Months 24 Months 36 Months 
Family Routines    
Bedtime Routine     
14 months -- .12*** .17*** 
24 months  -- .26*** 
36 months   -- 
Regular Bedtime     
14 months -- .20*** .15*** 
24 months  -- .26*** 
36 months   -- 
Reading Routines     
14 months -- .32*** .22*** 
24 months  -- .37*** 
36 months   -- 
Parent-Child Play    
14 months -- .47*** .44*** 
24 months  -- .55*** 
36 months   -- 
Parent-Child Outside Activity    
14 months -- .38*** .30*** 
24 months  -- .42*** 
36 months   -- 
General Parenting    
Discipline Severity Index     
14 months -- .36*** .31*** 
24 months  -- .40*** 
36 months   -- 
Language and Cognitive Stimulation     
14 months -- .45*** .41*** 
24 months  -- .45*** 
36 months   -- 
Maternal Supportiveness     
14 months -- .55*** .39*** 
24 months  -- .50*** 
36 months   -- 
***p  < .001  
 
 
 
104 
 
Table 8. 
Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Family Routines Measurement 
Model 
  
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
 
Variable Observed Variance Error Variance True Variance 
Reliability 
(True 
V/Obs V) 
Factor 
Indicator 
Correlation 
Unstd. 
Factor 
Loading 
Std. 
Factor 
Loading 
14-month Routines        
Bedtime Routine ? 
14m .21 .20 
.01 .05 .25 1.00 .25 
Regular Bedtime ? 
14m .24 .22 
.02 .08 .22 .95*** .22 
Reading Routines 
? 14m .25 .14 
.11 .44 .65 2.86*** .65 
Parent-Child Play 
? 14m .59 .23 
.36 .61 .78 5.25*** .78 
Parent-Child 
Outside Activity ? 
14m 
.40 .35 .05 .13 .35 1.93*** .35 
24-month Routines        
Bedtime Routine ? 
24m .22 .21 
.01 .05 .15 1.00 .15 
Regular Bedtime ? 
24m .24 .23 
.01 .04 .20 1.45*** .20 
Reading Routines 
? 24m .24 .13 
.11 .46 .69 5.10*** .69 
Parent-Child Play 
? 24m .65 .14 
.51 .78 .89 10.59*** .89 
Parent-Child 
Outside Activity ? 
24m 
.52 .46 .06 .11 .33 3.52*** .33 
36-month Routines        
Bedtime Routine ? 
36m .21 .21 
0 0 .14 1.00 .14 
Regular Bedtime ? 
36m .25 .24 
.01 .04 .14 1.05*** .14 
Reading Routines 
? 36m .25 .14 
.11 .44 .65 4.97*** .65 
Parent-Child Play 
? 36m .64 .20 
.44 .69 .83 10.29*** .83 
Parent-Child 
Outside Activity ? 
36m 
.48 .41 .07 .15 .36 3.78*** .36 
***p  < .001   
 
 
 
105 
 
Table 9.  
Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Parenting Measurement Model 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
 
Variable Observed Variance Error Variance True Variance 
Reliability 
(True 
V/Obs V) 
Factor 
Indicator 
Correlation 
Unstd. 
Factor 
Loading 
Std. 
Factor 
Loading 
14-month Parenting        
Maternal 
Supportiveness ? 
14m 
1.06 .65 .41 .39 .62 1.00 .62 
Discipline Severity 
Index ? 14m 2.34 2.12 .22 .09 -.30 -.72
*** -.30 
Language and 
Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 14m 
2.40 1.89 .51 .21 .46 1.12*** .46 
24-month Parenting        
Maternal 
Supportiveness ? 
24m 
1.08 .74 .34 .31 .56 1.00 .56 
Discipline Severity 
Index ? 24m 2.85 2.50 .35 .12 -.35 -1.01
*** -.35 
Language and 
Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 24m 
1.85 1.43 .42 .23 .48 1.11*** .48 
36-month Parenting        
Maternal 
Supportiveness ? 
36m 
.83 .59 .24 .29 .53 1.00 .53 
Discipline Severity 
Index ? 36m 2.61 2.05 .56 .21 -.46 -1.54
*** -.46 
Language and 
Cognitive 
Stimulation ? 36m 
3.49 2.25 1.24 .36 .60 2.29*** .60 
 
 
106 
 
Table 10. 
Estimated Psychometric Properties and Factor Loadings for Social Outcomes Measurement 
Model   
 
Variable Observed Variance Error Variance True Variance 
Reliability 
(True 
V/Obs V) 
Factor 
Indicator 
Correlation 
Unstd. 
Factor 
Loading 
Std. 
Factor 
Loading 
BRS:Orientation/ 
Engagement  .47 .11 .36 .77 .33 1.00 .33 
BRS: Emotion 
Regulation  .45 .40 
.05 .11 .88 2.74+ .88 
Child Behavior 
Checklist  
(Aggressive 
behavior subscale) 
38.18 37.22 .96 .03 -.16 -4.51*** -.16 
***p  < .001   
 
***p  < .001  +p  < .10   
 
 
 
107 
 
Table 11.  
Regression Model for Social Outcomes (standardized results) 
 
Variables Research Question 1 Research Question 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 
Routines       
14-months .25*** 
(.05) 
.17** 
(.07) 
.10 
(.07) 
-.51** 
(.16) 
.99 
(1.85) 
-.32* 
(.15) 
24-months  .15* 
(.06) 
.07 
(.07)  
-.93 
(1.20) 
.06 
(.11) 
36-months   .19** 
(.07)   
-.11 
(.10) 
Parenting       
14-months    1.13*** 
(.21) 
-2.44 
(4.39) 
.30* 
(.12) 
24-months     3.04 
(3.91) 
-.19 
(.12) 
36-months      .79*** 
(.18) 
Covariates       
Program Group 
(Early Head Start 
Program Status) 
.02 
(.04) 
.01 
(.04) 
.004 
(.04) 
-.05 
(.06) 
-.08 
(.13) 
-.06 
(.05) 
Male -.17*** 
(.04) 
-.16*** 
(.05) 
-.15*** 
(.05) 
-.14* 
(.06) 
-.05 
(.16) 
-.14** 
(.05) 
Black -.09 
(.06) 
-.09 
(.06) 
-.08 
(.06) 
.34** 
(.12) 
.19 
(.28) 
.17+ 
(.10) 
Hispanic -.03 
(.05) 
-.03 
(.05) 
-.02 
(.05) 
.17* 
(.07) 
-.20 
(.46) 
.02 
(.06) 
High School 
Diploma or GED 
.07 
(.05) 
.07 
(.05) 
.08+ 
(.05) 
-.20* 
(.08) 
-.11 
(.19) 
-.16* 
(.08) 
Not Married or 
Cohabitating 
-.10* 
(.05) 
-.10* 
(.05) 
-.10* 
(.05) 
.05 
(.07) 
-.21 
(.34) 
.03 
(.06) 
Maternal Depression -.07 
(.06) 
-.08 
(.06) 
-.07 
(.06) 
.10 
(.09) 
.16 
(.23) 
-.13+ 
(.07) 
Income-To-Needs .04 
(.05) 
.03 
(.05) 
.02 
(.05) 
-.01 
(.06) 
-.18 
(.26) 
-.01 
(.06) 
Inadequate Money -.04 
(.05) 
-.04 
(.04) 
-.04 
(.04) 
-.05 
(.06) 
-.07 
(.13) 
-.04 
(.05) 
Fit Statistics       
?2 (df) 1324.31*** 
(458) 
1318.99*** 
(457) 
1311.37*** 
(456) 
1267.75*** 
(457) 
1263.69*** 
(455) 
1427.91*** 
(454) 
??2 (df)  5.32*(1) 7.62**(1) 56.56***(1) 55.30***(1) 116.54 (2) 
Comparison  Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 
CFI .88 .88 .88 .89 .89 .87 
RMSEA .039, p=1.00 .039, p=1.00 .039, p=1.00 .038, p=1.00 .038, p=1.00 .038, p=1.00 
 
  
 
 
108 
 
Table 12.  
Regression Model for Cognitive Outcomes (standardized results) 
Variables Research Question 1 Research Question 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 
Routines       
14-months .23*** 
(.03) 
.11** 
(.05) 
.09* 
(.05) 
-.52*** 
(.12) 
-.92 
(.68) 
-.15 
(.11) 
24-months  .17*** 
(.04) 
.15*** 
(.04)  
.25 
(.45) 
.06 
(.08) 
36-months   .07 
(.04)   
-.22** 
(.07) 
Parenting       
14-months    1.11*** 
(.15) 
2.14 
(1.63) 
.04 
(.10) 
24-months     -.89 
(1.43) 
-.15+ 
(.09) 
36-months      .87*** 
(.12) 
Covariates       
Program Group 
(Early Head Start 
Program Status) 
.06* 
(.03) 
.05+ 
(.03) 
.04+ 
(.03) 
-.02 
(.04) 
-.003 
(.08) 
-.01 
(.04) 
Male -.08** 
(.03) 
-.08** 
(.03) 
-.07** 
(.03) 
-.06 
(.04) 
-.08 
(.08) 
-.07* 
(.03) 
Black -.25*** 
(.03) 
-.23*** 
(.03) 
-.22*** 
(.03) 
.21* 
(.09) 
.24 
(.16) 
-.05 
(.07) 
Hispanic -.18*** 
(.03) 
-.18*** 
(.03) 
-.18*** 
(.03) 
.01 
(.06) 
.12 
(.19) 
-.19*** 
(.04) 
High School 
Diploma or GED 
.17*** 
(.03) 
.16*** 
(.03) 
.17*** 
(.03) 
-.10 
(.06) 
-.11 
(.11) 
-.03 
(.06) 
Not Married or 
Cohabitating 
-.03 
(.03) 
-.04 
(.03) 
-.04 
(.03) 
.10* 
(.05) 
.17 
(.15) 
.06 
(.04) 
Maternal Depression -.12** 
(.04) 
-.12** 
(.04) 
-.12** 
(.04) 
.06 
(.07) 
.04 
(.13) 
-.18*** 
(.05) 
Income-To-Needs .01 
(.03) 
.01 
(.03) 
.01 
(.03) 
-.02 
(.05) 
.03 
(.12) 
-.02 
(.04) 
Inadequate Money -.01 
(.03) 
-.01 
(.03) 
-.01 
(.03) 
-.03 
(.05) 
-.03 
(.08) 
-.01 
(.04) 
Fit Statistics       
?2 (df) 1241.44*** 
(392) 
1222.67*** 
(391) 
1220.49*** 
(390) 
1075.70*** 
(391) 
1074.50*** 
(389) 
1243.16*** 
(388) 
??2 (df)  18.77***(1) 2.18*(1) 165.74***(1) 148.17***(1) 22.67 (2) 
Comparison  Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 
CFI .88 .89 .89 .91 .91 .88 
RMSEA .042, p=1.00 .042, p=1.00 .042, p=1.00 .038, p=1.00 .038, p=1.00 .042, p=1.00  
 
 
 
 
 
109 
 
 
 
 
  
Table
 13.
 
Regres
sion Model
 for Obser
ved I
nd
icators of
 Ro
uti
nes a
t 36
-months
 (standardi
zed r
esult
s) 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
Observed
 
Variables
 
So
cial Outco
me
s
 
Cog
ni
tiv
e Ou
tco
m
es
 
Mo
del 
1
 
Mo
del 
2
 
Mo
del 
3
 
Mo
del 
4
 
Mo
del 
5
 
Mo
del 
6
 
Mo
del 
7
 
Mo
del 
8
 
Mo
del 
9
 
Mo
del 
10
 
Bedti
m
e 
Rou
tin
e
 
.1
1
*
 
.0
9
*
 
.0
8
+
 
.0
8
+
 
.0
8
+
 
.0
6
*
 
.0
6
*
 
.0
5
+
 
.0
5
+
 
.0
5
+
 
Regu
lar 
Bedti
m
e
 
 
.0
9
*
 
.0
8
+
 
.0
8
+
 
.0
8
+
 
 
.0
1
 
-
.0
01
 
-
.0
02
 
-
.0
01
 
Rea
di
ng
 
Rou
tin
es
 
 
 
.1
2
**
 
.0
8
 
.0
8
 
 
 
.1
1
*
*
*
 
.1
1
*
*
*
 
.1
1
*
*
*
 
Parent
-
Chi
ld
 
Play
 
 
 
 
.1
0
+
 
.0
4
 
 
 
 
.0
1
 
.0
2
 
Parent
-
Chi
ld
 
Outsid
e 
Activit
y
 
 
 
 
 
.1
6
*
*
*
 
 
 
 
 
-
.0
1
 
Fit 
St
at
istics
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
?
2
 
(d
f)
 
13
38.3
7
*
*
*
 
(4
58
)
 
13
33.8
0
*
*
*
 
(4
57
)
 
13
26.3
6
*
*
*
 
(4
56
)
 
13
22.6
1
*
*
*
 
(4
55
)
 
13
11.1
5
*
*
*
 
(4
54
)
 
12
78.5
9
*
*
*
 
(3
92
)
 
12
78.4
6
*
*
*
 
(3
91
)
 
12
61.1
3
*
*
*
 
(3
90
)
 
12
60.9
3
*
*
*
 
(3
89
)
 
12
60.8
2
*
*
*
 
(3
88
)
 
??
2
 
(d
f)
 
 
4.
57
*
 
(1
)
 
7.
44
*
*
 
(1
)
 
3.
75
 (
1)
 
11
.46
**
*
 
(1
)
 
 
.1
3
 
(1
)
 
17
.33
***
 
(1
)
 
.2
 (1
)
 
.1
1 
(1
)
 
Co
m
pariso
n
 
 
Mo
del 
1
 
Mo
del 
2
 
Mo
del 
3
 
Mo
del 
4
 
 
Mo
del 
6
 
Mo
del 
7
 
Mo
del 
8
 
Mo
del 
9
 
CFI
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
.8
8
 
RMSE
A
 
.0
42
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
39
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
39
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
39
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
39
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
43
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
43
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
43
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
43
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
.0
43
,
 
p
=1.0
0
 
 
 
 
110 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Table
 14.
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for G
ender
 and Social
 Outc
omes (
unst
andardi
zed r
esult
s) 
 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
14
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
24
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
36
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
2
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
4
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
6
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
8
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
1
0
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
11
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
2
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
G
irl
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
2
1
6
)
 
.
9
6
**
 
(
.
3
1
8
)
 
.
5
7
**
 
(
.
1
9
4
)
 
1
.
4
7
*
 
(
.
6
4
5
)
 
.
9
7
**
 
(
.
3
0
9
)
 
.
8
8
*
 
(
.
3
7
8
)
 
1
.
0
6
**
 
(
3
5
1
)
 
.
8
9
*
 
(
.
4
2
3
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
3
5
1
)
 
.
9
0
*
 
(
.
3
6
3
)
 
1
.
2
0
***
  
(
.
3
5
0
)
 
.
7
7
*
 
(
.
3
2
0
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
8
7
***
 
(
.
1
6
6
)
 
1
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
1
8
0
)
 
1
.
4
3
***
 
(
.
3
5
6
)
 
1
.
7
7
**
 
(
.
5
7
2
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
4
5
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
6
0
)
 
1
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
4
1
2
)
 
1
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
3
9
0
)
 
1
.
1
4
 
***
 
(
.
2
4
6
)
 
.
9
9
***
 
(
.
2
3
0
)
 
.
8
7
**
 
(
.
3
0
9
)
 
.
7
6
**
 
(
.
2
4
8
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
3
.
0
8
***
 
(
.
4
5
1
)
 
2
.
8
0
***
 
(
.
4
0
1
)
 
3
.
4
5
***
 
(
.
7
9
8
)
 
4
.
4
2
**
 
(
1
.
4
2
)
 
4
.
2
4
**
*
 
(
7
.
3
4
)
 
4
.
3
7
***
 
(
.
8
0
3
)
 
4
.
9
9
***
 
(
1
.
2
9
)
 
4
.
6
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
4
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
7
5
7
)
 
4
.
2
7
***
 
(
.
7
6
4
)
 
4
.
5
4
***
  
(
1
.
1
3
)
 
3
.
8
0
***
  
(
8
.
1
6
)
 
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
6
5
0
)
 
4
.
6
1
***
 
(
7
.
1
6
)
 
5
.
6
7
***
 
(
1
.
2
8
)
 
7
.
1
9
**
 
(
2
.
6
2
)
 
7
.
0
8
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
4
1
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
8
.
0
1
***
 
(
2
.
0
8
)
 
7
.
4
4
***
 
(
1
.
9
9
)
 
7
.
5
6
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
6
.
7
5
***
 
(
1
.
2
2
)
 
6
.
6
1
***
  
(
1
.
6
4
)
 
5
.
4
8
***
  
(
1
.
1
8
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
 
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
4
4
***
 
(
.
3
8
1
)
 
1
.
6
4
***
 
(
2
.
8
5
)
 
2
.
3
2
***
 
(
.
6
1
2
)
 
2
.
8
7
**
 
(
.
9
7
7
)
 
2
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
4
5
3
)
 
2
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
4
9
4
)
 
2
.
8
3
***
 
(
.
8
7
0
)
 
2
.
6
3
**
 
(
.
8
3
0
)
 
1
.
9
4
***
 
(
.
4
5
1
)
 
2
.
5
6
***
 
(
.
5
2
9
)
 
3
.
0
5
***
  
(
.
8
2
3
)
 
2
.
5
2
***
  
(
.
6
1
0
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
B
o
y
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
2
1
6
)
 
.
5
2
*
 
(
.
2
3
3
)
 
.
5
7
**
 
(
.
1
9
4
)
 
.
4
0
*
 
(
.
1
7
6
)
 
.
9
7
**
 
(
.
3
0
9
)
 
1
.
2
2
**
 
(
.
4
3
8
)
 
1
.
0
6
**
 
(
.
3
5
1
)
 
1
.
2
1
*
 
(
.
4
8
6
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
3
5
1
)
 
1
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
4
4
6
)
 
1
.
2
0
***
  
(
.
3
5
0
)
 
1
.
8
6
*
 
(
.
7
2
9
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
111 
 
  
    
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
8
7
***
 
(
.
1
6
6
)
 
1
.
0
6
***
 
(
1
.
8
0
)
 
.
7
7
***
 
(
.
1
7
8
)
 
.
7
3
***
 
(
.
1
6
0
)
 
1
.
2
2
 
(
.
7
3
5
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
6
0
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
3
1
9
)
 
1
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
3
6
4
)
 
1
.
1
4
***
 
(
.
2
4
6
)
 
.
9
9
***
 
(
.
2
3
0
)
 
1
.
1
1
***
  
(
.
3
1
8
)
 
1
.
3
6
**
 
(
.
4
9
2
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
3
.
0
8
***
 
(
.
4
5
1
)
 
2
.
8
0
***
 
(
.
4
0
1
)
 
2
.
2
5
***
 
(
.
3
6
6
)
 
2
.
1
0
***
 
(
.
3
1
1
)
 
4
.
2
4
***
 
(
.
7
3
5
)
 
4
.
3
7
***
 
(
.
8
0
3
)
 
3
.
9
3
***
 
(
.
8
5
4
)
 
4
.
1
6
***
 
(
1
.
0
7
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
7
5
7
)
 
4
.
2
7
***
 
(
.
7
6
4
)
 
4
.
0
7
***
  
(
1
.
0
3
)
 
5
.
1
3
**
 
(
1
.
6
3
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
6
5
0
)
 
4
.
6
1
***
 
(
.
6
4
4
)
 
3
.
7
7
***
 
(
.
6
0
3
)
 
3
.
5
0
***
 
(
.
5
1
0
)
 
7
.
0
8
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
4
1
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
7
.
2
2
***
 
(
1
.
6
2
)
 
7
.
6
4
***
 
(
2
.
0
1
)
 
7
.
5
6
***
 
(
1
.
2
6
)
 
6
.
7
5
***
 
(
1
.
2
2
)
 
7
.
6
3
***
  
(
1
.
9
9
)
 
8
.
8
6
**
 
(
2
.
8
4
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
4
4
***
 
(
.
3
8
1
)
 
1
.
6
4
***
 
(
.
2
8
5
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
6
9
)
 
1
.
1
4
***
 
(
.
2
3
9
)
 
2
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
4
5
3
)
 
2
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
4
9
4
)
 
1
.
7
2
***
 
(
.
5
4
1
)
 
1
.
8
2
**
 
(
.
6
2
4
)
 
2
.
1
0
***
 
(
.
4
6
1
)
 
2
.
5
6
***
 
(
.
5
2
9
)
 
1
.
8
6
**
 
(
.
6
2
7
)
 
2
.
7
4
**
 
(
.
9
9
9
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
2
**
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
F
it S
ta
tis
tics
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
?
2
 
(
df
)
 
2
6
2
6
.
8
1
**
*
 
(
9
3
3
)
 
1
8
6
5
.
0
3
**
*
 
(
9
1
6
)
 
1
8
3
6
.
1
3
**
*
 
(
8
9
9
)
 
1
8
3
0
.
8
5
**
*
 
(
8
9
8
)
 
2
6
2
7
.
7
4
**
*
 
(
9
3
3
)
 
1
8
6
7
.
3
8
**
*
 
(
9
1
6
)
 
1
8
3
3
.
5
9
**
*
 
(
8
9
9
)
 
1
8
3
3
.
2
9
**
*
 
(
8
9
8
)
 
2
6
2
2
.
2
1
**
*
 
(
9
3
3
)
 
1
8
5
8
.
6
6
**
*
  
(
9
1
6
)
 
1
8
2
7
.
6
6
**
*
 
(
8
9
9
)
 
1
8
2
4
.
6
6
**
*
 
(
8
9
8
)
 
??
2
 
(
df
)
 
 
7
6
1
.
7
8
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
0
.
6
8
***
 
(
3
4
)
 
5.
28
*
 
(
1
)
 
 
7
6
0
.
3
6
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
4
.
1
5
***
 
(
2
3
)
 
.
3
 
(
1
)
 
 
7
6
3
.
5
5
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
4
.
5
5
*** 
(
3
4
)
 
3
.
0
0
 
(
1
)
 
C
o
m
p
ar
is
o
n
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
C
FI
 
.
7
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
7
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
7
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
R
MSE
A
 
(
C
I
)
 
.
0
5
4
 
(
.
0
5
2
 
to
 
 
.0
57
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
054
 
(
.
0
5
2
 
to
 
 
.
0
5
7
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
054
 
(
.
0
5
2
 
to
 
 
.
0
5
7
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
.
0
4
1
 
(
.
0
3
8
 
to
 
.
0
4
4
)
 
Table
 14 
(co
nt.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for G
ender
 and Social
 Outc
omes
 
 *
**
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
112 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
Table
 15.
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for G
ender
 and Cogniti
ve O
utc
omes 
(unstandardi
zed res
ults)
 
 
 
 
14
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
24
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
36
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
2
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
4
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
6
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
8
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
e
d
 
Mo
d
el 
1
0
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
2
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
G
irl
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
2
.
4
5
***
 
(
4
.
7
1
)
 
2
5
.
1
1
***
 
(
6
.
1
3
)
 
2
0
.
7
6
***
 
(
4
.
2
7
)
 
3
7
.
4
5
**
 
(
1
3
.
7
0
)
 
2
9
.
7
3
***
 
(
6
.
2
5
)
 
3
1
.
3
2
***
 
(
7
.
8
4
)
 
3
1
.
8
4
**
 
(
6
.
8
9
)
 
3
1
.
8
5
***
 
(9
.
7
7
)
 
2
8
.
8
5
***
 
(
6
.
8
8
)
 
2
2
.
5
8
**
 
(
7
.
3
7
)
 
2
9
.
6
5
***
  
(
7
.
1
0
)
 
1
9
.
2
7
**
 
(
6
.
6
3
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
8
9
***
 
(
.
1
6
7
)
 
1
.
0
8
***
 
(
.
1
8
1
)
 
1
.
4
0
***
 
(
.
3
1
5
)
 
1
.
7
8
***
 
(
.
5
6
0
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
3
1
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
2
4
2
)
 
1
.
1
9
***
 
(
.
3
3
4
)
 
1
.
1
9
***
 
(
.
3
6
4
)
 
1
.
1
2
 
***
 
(
.
2
4
3
)
 
.
9
8
***
 
(
.
2
2
8
)
 
.
8
8
**
 
(
.
3
2
0
)
 
.
7
5
**
 
(
.
2
4
8
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
3
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
4
4
5
)
 
2
.
7
4
***
 
(
.
3
9
4
)
 
3
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
6
6
4
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
1
.
3
3
)
 
4
.
1
0
***
 
(
.
6
9
0
)
 
4
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
7
4
5
)
 
4
.
4
5
***
 
(
.
9
5
9
)
 
4
.
4
5
***
 
(
1
.
1
4
)
 
4
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
7
5
3
)
 
4
.
2
8
***
 
(
.
7
6
6
)
 
4
.
7
5
***
  
(
1
.
1
8
)
 
3
.
8
3
***
  
(
.
8
2
7
)
 
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
3
8
***
 
(
.
6
3
2
)
 
4
.
4
4
***
 
(
.
6
1
9
)
 
5
.
2
5
***
 
(
1
.
0
5
)
 
6
.
8
0
***
 
(
2
.
1
0
)
 
6
.
7
1
***
 
(
1
.
1
4
)
 
6
.
9
8
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
0
6
***
 
(
1
.
5
3
)
 
7
.
0
6
***
 
(
1
.
8
2
)
 
7
.
3
9
***
 
(
1
.
3
4
)
 
6
.
6
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
0
)
 
6
.
8
3
***
  
(
1
.
6
9
)
 
5
.
4
8
***
  
(
1
.
1
8
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
 
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
3
9
***
 
(
.
3
7
3
)
 
1
.
5
9
***
 
(
.
2
7
7
)
 
2
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
5
2
2
)
 
2
.
7
5
**
 
(
.
9
2
2
)
 
1
.
9
6
***
 
(
.
4
2
1
)
 
2
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
4
5
5
)
 
2
.
4
8
***
 
(
.
6
7
4
)
 
2
.
4
8
***
 
(
.
7
6
4
)
 
1
.
8
0
***
 
(
.
4
2
6
)
 
2
.
4
3
***
 
(
.
5
0
3
)
 
3
.
0
9
***
  
(
.
8
3
5
)
 
2
.
4
7
***
  
(
.
6
0
3
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
-
2
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
B
o
y
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
2
.
4
5
***
 
(
4
.
7
1
)
 
2
0
.
0
4
***
 
(
5
.
5
5
)
 
2
0
.
7
6
***
 
(
4
.
2
7
)
 
1
5
.
6
6
***
 
(
4
.
1
8
)
 
2
9
.
7
3
***
 
(
6
.
2
5
)
 
3
2
.
4
4
***
 
(
8
.
4
2
)
 
3
1
.
8
4
**
 
(
6
.
8
9
)
 
3
1
.
8
3
***
 
(9
.
7
3
)
 
2
8
.
8
5
***
 
(
6
.
8
8
)
 
3
6
.
1
6
***
 
(
9
.
2
0
)
 
2
9
.
6
5
***
  
(
7
.
1
0
)
 
4
4
.
3
7
**
 
(
1
5
.
8
0
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
113 
 
 
 
  
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 Table
 15 (c
ont.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for G
ender
 and Cogniti
ve O
utc
omes
 
 
   
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
8
9
***
 
(
.
1
6
7
)
 
1
.
0
8
***
 
(
.
1
8
1
)
 
.
8
1
***
 
(
.
1
8
3
)
 
.
7
4
***
 
(
.
1
6
2
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
3
1
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
2
4
2
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
3
1
5
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
3
3
6
)
 
1
.
1
2
 
***
 
(
.
2
4
3
)
 
.
9
8
***
 
(
.
2
2
8
)
 
1
.
0
9
***
  
(
.
3
0
7
)
 
1
.
3
4
**
 
(
.
4
8
1
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
3
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
4
4
5
)
 
2
.
7
5
***
 
(
.
3
9
4
)
 
2
.
2
9
***
 
(
1
.
3
7
)
 
2
.
0
9
*
**
 
(
.
3
1
2
)
 
4
.
1
0
***
 
(
.
6
9
0
)
 
4
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
7
4
5
)
 
4
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
8
4
4
)
 
4
.
0
2
***
 
(
.
9
9
8
)
 
4
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
7
5
3
)
 
4
.
2
8
***
 
(
.
7
6
6
)
 
4
.
0
1
***
  
(
.
8
2
3
)
 
5
.
1
0
***
 
(
1
.
6
0
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
3
8
***
 
(
.
6
3
2
)
 
4
.
4
4
***
 
(
.
6
1
9
)
 
3
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
5
9
3
)
 
3
.
4
0
***
 
(
.
4
9
5
)
 
6
.
7
1
***
 
(
1
.
1
4
)
 
6
.
9
8
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
0
8
***
 
(
1
.
5
2
)
 
7
.
0
8
***
 
(
.
1
.
7
8
)
 
7
.
3
9
***
 
(
1
.
3
4
)
 
6
.
6
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
0
)
 
6
.
7
1
***
  
(
1
.
3
9
)
 
8
.
5
2
**
 
(
2
.
6
9
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
3
9
***
 
(
.
3
7
3
)
 
1
.
5
9
***
 
(
.
2
7
7
)
 
1
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
2
6
9
)
 
1
.
1
0
***
 
(
.
2
3
5
)
 
1
.
7
1
**
 
(
.
5
6
9
)
 
2
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
4
5
5
)
 
1
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
5
2
4
)
 
1
.
1
3
***
 
(
.
2
3
7
)
 
1
.
8
0
***
 
(
.
4
2
6
)
 
2
.
4
3
***
 
(
.
5
0
3)
 
1
.
9
4
***
 
(
.
5
4
0
)
 
2
.
4
6
**
 
(
.
9
0
3
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
*
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
+
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
2
**
 
(
.
0
0
7
)
 
-
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
F
it S
ta
tis
tics
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
?
2
 
(
df
)
 
2
4
5
0
.
9
4
**
*
 
(
8
0
1
)
 
1
6
8
8
.
5
0
**
*
 
(
7
8
4
)
 
1
6
5
8
.
7
7
 
(
7
6
7
)
 
1
6
5
4
.
2
6
**
*
 
(
7
6
6
)
 
2
4
4
0
.
2
8
**
*
 
(
8
0
1
)
 
1
6
7
6
.
9
6
**
*
 
(
7
8
4
)
 
1
6
4
3
.
8
2
**
*
 
(
7
6
7
)
 
1
6
4
3
.
8
2
**
*
 
(
7
6
6
)
 
2
4
5
4
.
6
3
**
*
 
(
8
0
1
)
 
1
6
8
8
.
4
9
**
*
  
(
7
8
4
)
 
1
6
5
7
.
4
0
**
*
 
(
7
6
7
)
 
1
6
5
4
.
2
7
**
*
 
(
7
6
6
)
 
??
2
 
(
df
)
 
 
7
6
2
.
4
4
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
2
.
1
7
***
 
(
3
4
)
 
4
.
5
1
*
 
(
1
)
 
 
7
6
3
.
3
2
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
6
.
4
6
***
 
(
3
4
)
 
0
 
(
1
)
 
 
7
6
6
.
1
4
***
 
(
1
7
)
 
7
9
7
.
2
3
***
 
(
3
4
)
 
3
.
1
3
 
(
1
)
 
C
o
m
p
ar
is
o
n
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Mo
d
el
 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
C
FI
 
.
7
7
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
7
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
7
7
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
.
8
8
 
R
MSE
A
 
(
C
I
)
 
.
0
5
8
 
(
.
0
5
5
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
0
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
1
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
1
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
058
 
(
.
0
5
5
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
0
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
058
 
(
.
0
5
5
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
1
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
1
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
1
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
 
 
114 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
 
 
14
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
24
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
36
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
2
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
4
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
6
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
8
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
1
0
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
2
 
 
A
ll
 
Fre
e
 
White
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
2
1
1
)
 
.
6
5
*
 
(
.
2
6
4
)
 
.
5
5
**
 
(
.
1
9
0
)
 
.
6
8
*
 
(
.
2
9
9
)
 
.
9
5
**
 
(
.
3
2
2
)
 
.
7
6
*
 
(
.
3
4
0
)
 
.
7
2
**
 
(
.
2
7
3
)
 
1
.
3
8
 
(
.
8
9
9
)
 
1
.
2
5
***
 
(
.
3
6
2
)
 
.
9
1
*
 
(
.
3
7
1
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
  
(
.
3
4
0
)
 
1
.
0
1
*
 
(
.
4
7
9
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
3
*
**
 
(
.
1
6
8
)
 
1
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
1
7
5
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
2
2
7
)
 
1
.
2
5
***
 
(
.
2
8
5
)
 
1
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
2
6
7
)
 
1
.
2
7
***
 
(
.
2
7
1
)
 
2
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
6
4
7
)
 
2
.
8
9
*
 
(
1
.
4
3
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
2
***
 
(
.
2
3
3
)
 
1
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
4
2
0
)
 
1
.
1
5
**
 
(
.
4
1
9
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
9
8
***
 
(
.
4
3
0
)
 
2
.
7
6
***
 
(
.
3
9
0
)
 
2
.
8
8
***
 
(
.
4
9
8
)
 
3
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
6
5
5
)
 
4
.
3
1
***
 
(
.
7
8
1
)
 
4
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
8
0
3
)
 
6
.
0
5
***
 
(
1
.
7
6
)
 
8
.
0
5
*
 
(
3
.
9
4
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
7
7
2
)
 
4
.
1
8
***
 
(
.
7
4
6
)
 
5
.
0
9
***
  
(
1
.
3
0
)
 
4
.
2
4
***
  
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
6
3
7
)
 
4
.
6
0
***
 
(
.
6
3
6
)
 
4
.
5
7
***
 
(
.
7
7
3
)
 
4
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
9
7
2
)
 
7
.
5
4
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
7
.
6
9
***
 
(
1
.
4
6
)
 
1
1
.
6
7
***
 
(
3
.
5
0
)
 
1
6
.
3
2
*
 
(
8
.
1
4
)
 
7
.
5
7
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
6
.
8
3
***
 
(
1
.
2
4
)
 
1
0
.
2
8
***
  
(
2
.
7
0
)
 
8
.
7
1
***
  
(
2
.
6
5
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
 
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
3
5
6
)
 
1
.
6
9
***
 
(
.
2
8
5
)
 
1
.
4
7
***
 
(
.
3
1
5
)
 
1
.
6
0
***
 
(
.
3
9
3
)
 
2
.
3
6
***
 
(
.
5
2
5
)
 
2
.
3
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
6
)
 
3
.
6
6
**
 
(
1
.
2
1
)
 
5
.
1
7
+
 
(
2
.
7
1
)
 
2
.
4
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
4
)
 
2
.
7
7
***
 
(
.
5
5
1
)
 
4
.
4
0
***
  
(
1
.
1
9
)
 
3
.
6
5
**
  
(
1
.
1
7
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Af
rica
n 
A
m
er
ica
n
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
2
1
1
)
 
.
7
1
**
 
(
.
2
6
1
)
 
.
5
5
***
 
(
.
1
9
0
)
 
.
6
5
*
 
(
.
2
7
5
)
 
.
9
5
**
 
(
.
3
2
2
)
 
.
9
7
***
 
(
.
4
1
0
)
 
.
7
2
**
 
(
.
2
7
3
)
 
.
6
2
*
 
(
.
2
6
7
)
 
1
.
2
5
***
 
(
.
3
6
2
)
 
1
.
5
0
***
 
(
.
4
6
7
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
  
(
.
3
4
0
)
 
1
.
1
8
**
 
(
.
4
0
7
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
Table
 16.
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for R
ace/
Ethn
icit
y and Soci
al Outcomes (unstandardized 
results
) 
 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < 
.05
   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
115 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
   
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
3
***
 
(
.
1
6
8
)
 
1
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
1
7
5
)
 
.
8
3
***
 
(
.
2
5
6
)
 
.
8
5
8
**
 
(
.
2
7
1
)
 
1
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
2
6
7
)
 
1
.
2
7
***
 
(
.
2
7
1
)
 
.
7
2
***
 
(
.
2
2
1
)
 
.
7
1
***
 
(
.
2
1
2
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
2
***
 
(
.
2
3
3
)
 
.
7
5
**
  
(
.
2
6
0
)
 
.
7
6
**
 
(
.
2
6
9
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
9
8
***
 
(
.
4
3
0
)
 
2
.
7
6
***
 
(
.
3
9
0
)
 
2
.
6
1
***
 
(
.
5
5
5
)
 
2
.
6
7
***
 
(
.
6
2
5
)
 
4
.
3
1
***
 
(
.
7
8
1
)
 
4
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
8
0
3
)
 
3
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
6
5
1
)
 
2
.
9
5
***
 
(
.
6
1
0
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
7
7
2
)
 
4
.
1
8
***
 
(
.
7
4
6
)
 
4
.
1
6
***
  
(
.
9
7
2
)
 
4
.
2
2
***
 
(
1
.
0
8
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
6
3
7
)
 
4
.
6
0
***
 
(
.
6
3
6
)
 
4
.
7
7
***
 
(
1
.
0
0
)
 
4
.
8
7
***
 
(
1
.
1
2
)
 
7
.
5
4
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
7
.
6
9
***
 
(
1
.
4
6
)
 
4
.
8
7
***
 
(
1
.
0
3
)
 
4
.
6
7
***
 
(
.
9
6
6
)
 
7
.
5
7
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
6
.
8
3
***
 
(
1
.
2
4)
 
4
.
8
2
***
  
(
1
.
0
6
)
 
4
.
8
9
***
  
(
1
.
1
8
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
3
5
6
)
 
1
.
6
9
***
 
(
.
2
8
5
)
 
2
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
5
6
6
)
 
2
.
2
8
***
 
(
.
6
1
0
)
 
2
.
3
6
***
 
(
.
5
2
5
)
 
2
.
3
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
6
)
 
1
.
4
7
**
 
(
.
4
7
7
)
 
1
.
4
0
**
 
(
.
4
5
0
)
 
2
.
4
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
4
)
 
2
.
7
7
***
 
(
.
5
5
1
)
 
1
.
4
7
**
 
(
.
4
7
8
)
 
1
.
4
8
**
  
(
.
5
0
9
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
  
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
  
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
  
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
  
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
H
is
pa
nic
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
2
1
1
)
 
-
.
4
8
 
(
.
6
0
7
)
 
.
5
5
***
 
(
.
1
9
0
)
 
-
.
3
5
 
(
.
4
3
2
)
 
.
9
5
**
 
(
.
3
2
2
)
 
-
.
4
7
 
(
.
8
6
5
)
 
.
7
2
**
 
(
.
2
7
3
)
 
-
.
5
1
 
(
.
8
0
5
)
 
1
.
2
5
***
 
(
.
3
6
2
)
 
.
5
0
 
 
(
.
6
8
9
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
  
(
.
3
4
0
)
 
.
8
4
 
 
(
1
.
4
2
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
3
***
 
(
.
1
6
8
)
 
1
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
1
7
5
)
 
1
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
2
2
7
)
 
.
8
5
**
 
(
.
3
3
1
)
 
1
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
2
6
7
)
 
1
.
2
7
***
 
(
.
2
7
1
)
 
2
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
6
4
7
)
 
1
.
0
5
+
 
(
.
6
3
3
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
2
***
 
(
.
2
3
3
)
 
1
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
4
2
0
)
 
1
.
8
2
 
(
1
.
4
7
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
9
8
***
 
(
.
4
3
0
)
 
2
.
7
6
***
 
(
.
3
9
0
)
 
2
.
8
8
***
 
(
.
4
9
8
)
 
1
.
9
0
***
 
(
.
5
6
9
)
 
4
.
3
1
***
 
(
.
7
8
1
)
 
4
.
3
4
***
 
(
.
8
0
3
)
 
6
.
0
5
***
 
(
1
.
7
6
)
 
4
.
0
8
*
 
(
1
.
8
8
)
 
4
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
7
7
2
)
 
4
.
1
8
***
 
(
.
7
4
6
)
 
5
.
0
9
***
 
(
1
.
3
0
)
 
7
.
1
8
 
(
5
.
6
4
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
6
3
7
)
 
4
.
6
0
***
 
(
.
6
3
6
)
 
4
.
5
8
***
 
(
.
7
7
3
)
 
3
.
8
4
***
 
(
1
.
1
4
)
 
7
.
5
4
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
7
.
6
9
***
 
(
1
.
4
6
)
 
1
1
.
6
7
***
 
(
3
.
5
0
)
 
6
.
8
0
*
 
(
3
.
1
9
)
 
 
7
.
5
7
***
 
(
1
.
4
1
)
 
6
.
8
3
***
 
(
1
.
2
4
)
 
1
0
.
2
8
***
 
(
2
.
7
0
)
 
1
4
.
6
9
 
(
1
1
.
8
1
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
2
9
***
 
(
.
3
5
6
)
 
1
.
6
9
***
 
(
.
2
8
5
)
 
1
.
4
7
***
 
(
.
3
1
5
)
 
.
8
7
 
 
(
.
5
5
8
)
 
2
.
3
6
***
 
(
.
5
2
5
)
 
2
.
3
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
6
)
 
3
.
6
6
**
 
(
1
.
2
1
)
 
1
.
9
0
 
(
1
.
1
6
)
 
2
.
4
2
***
 
(
.
5
2
4
)
 
2
.
7
7
***
 
(
.
5
5
1
)
 
4
.
4
0
***
 
(
1
.
1
9
)
 
6
.
4
8
 
(
5
.
2
2
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
5
**
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
3
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
-
.
0
5
**
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
3
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
-
.
0
4
**
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
3
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
0
2
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
0
3
 
(
.
0
0
5
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
0
2
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
0
4
 
(
.
0
0
6
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
0
2
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
0
4
 
(
.
0
0
5
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
3
**
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
**
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
3
**
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
Table
 16 (c
ont.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for R
ace/
Ethn
icit
y and Soci
al Outcomes
 
 ***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
116 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 Table
 16 (c
ont.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for R
ace/
Ethn
icit
y and Soci
al Outcomes
 
 ***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
  F
it S
ta
tis
tics
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
?
2
 
(
df
)
 
2
9
5
2
.
2
5
**
*
 
(
1
3
6
2
)
 
2
3
2
6
.
3
3
**
*
 
(
1
3
3
4
)
 
2
1
8
1
.
8
3
**
*
 
(
1
3
1
8
)
 
2
1
5
6
.
3
2
**
*
 
(
1
2
9
8
)
 
2
9
5
4
.
6
5
**
*
 
(
1
3
6
2
)
 
2
3
2
9
.
2
3
**
*
 
(
1
3
3
4
)
 
2
1
8
2
.
9
8
**
*
 
(
1
3
1
8
)
 
2
1
5
8
.
5
0
**
*
 
(
1
2
9
8
)
 
2
9
4
7
.
0
4
**
*
 
(
1
3
6
2
)
 
2
3
1
9
.
2
5
**
*
  
(
1
3
3
4
)
 
2
1
7
0
.
9
7
**
*
 
(
1
3
1
8
)
 
2
1
4
8
.
2
7
**
*
 
(
1
2
9
8
)
 
??
2
 
(
df
)
 
 
6
2
5
.
9
2
***
 
 
(
2
8
)
 
7
7
0
.
4
2
***
 
(
4
4
)
 
2
5
.
5
1
 
(
2
0
)
 
 
6
2
5
.
4
2
***
 
(
2
8
)
 
7
7
1
.
6
7
***
 
(
4
4
)
 
2
4
.
4
8
 
(
2
0
)
 
 
6
2
7
.
7
9
***
 
(
2
8
)
 
7
7
6
.
0
7
***
 
(
4
4
)
 
2
2
.
7
0
 
(
2
0
)
 
C
o
m
p
ar
is
o
n
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
C
FI
 
.
7
5
 
.
8
4
 
.
8
6
 
.
8
7
 
.
75
 
.
8
4
 
.
8
6
 
.
8
6
 
.
7
5
 
.
8
5
 
.
8
7
 
.
8
7
 
R
MSE
A
 
(
C
I
)
 
.
0
5
4
 
(
.
0
5
1
 
to
 
 
.
0
5
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
to
 
.
0
4
3
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
to
 
.
0
4
3
)
 
054
 
(
.
0
5
1
 
to
 
 
.
0
5
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
to
 
.
0
4
3
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
to
 
.
0
4
3
)
 
054
 
(
.
0
5
1
 
to
 
 
.
0
5
6
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
t
o 
.
0
4
3
)
 
.
0
4
0
 
(
.
0
3
7
 
to
 
.
0
4
3
)
 
 
 
117 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
Table
 17.
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for R
ace/
Ethn
icit
y and Cognit
ive Out
comes (unstandardi
zed r
esults)
 
 ***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < 
.05
   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
  
 
14
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
24
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
36
-
Mo
n
th
 
Mo
d
els
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
2
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
4
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
ed
 
Mo
d
el 
6
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
7
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
8
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
 
A
ll 
 
F
ix
e
d
 
Mo
d
el 
1
0
 
 
R
eg
r
es
s
 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
 
Me
asu
r
e 
Fre
e
 
Mo
d
el 
1
2
 
 
A
ll 
 
Fre
e
 
White
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
2
.
3
3
***
 
(
4
.
5
7
)
 
2
6
.
8
6
***
 
(
6
.
4
1
)
 
2
0
.
4
6
***
 
(
4
.
6
0
)
 
2
7
.
4
6
***
 
(
7
.
7
9
)
 
3
0
.
9
1
***
 
(
6
.
6
5
)
 
3
0
.
1
6
***
 
(
8
.
2
6
)
 
IE
 
4
9
.
9
6
*
 
(
2
5
.
2
8
)
 
2
7
.
4
1
***
 
(
6
.
9
5
)
 
3
0
.
9
3
***
 
(
8
.
9
4
)
 
2
6
.
1
3
***
  
(
6
.
5
1
)
 
3
1
.
0
6
**
 
(
1
1
.
7
7
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
4
***
 
(
.
1
6
6
)
 
1
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
1
7
6
)
 
1
.
1
3
***
 
(
.
2
3
0
)
 
1
.
2
4
***
 
(
.
2
8
7
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
4
7
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
5
8
)
 
 
2
.
6
5
*
 
(
1
.
2
3
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
1
***
 
(
.
2
3
1
)
 
1
.
0
9
***
 
(
.
3
4
3
)
 
1
.
1
8
**
 
(
.
4
1
7
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
8
9
***
 
(
.
4
1
1
)
 
2
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
3
7
9
)
 
2
.
8
5
***
 
(
.
4
8
4
)
 
3
.
7
9
***
 
(
.
6
7
1
)
 
4
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
7
2
5
)
 
4
.
2
4
***
 
(
.
7
6
6
)
 
 
7
.
5
9
*
 
(
3
.
4
7
)
 
4
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
7
7
7
)
 
4
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
7
4
5
)
 
3
.
9
2
***
  
(
.
8
9
0
)
 
4
.
2
6
***
  
(
1
.
2
3
)
 
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
3
3
***
 
(
.
6
0
6
)
 
4
.
3
9
***
 
(
.
6
0
5
)
 
4
.
1
3
***
 
(
.
6
9
6
)
 
4
.
6
2
***
 
(
.
9
6
6
)
 
7
.
0
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
3
3
***
 
(
1
.
3
5
)
 
 
1
4
.
5
6
*
 
(
6
.
7
6
)
 
7
.
4
5
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
6
.
6
5
***
 
(
1
.
2
0
)
 
7
.
4
3
***
  
(
1
.
7
6
)
 
8
.
0
4
***
  
(
2
.
3
7
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
 
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
3
4
3
)
 
1
.
5
8
***
 
(
.
2
7
2
)
 
1
.
4
1
***
 
(
.
3
0
6
)
 
1
.
5
7
***
 
(
.
3
9
2
)
 
2
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
4
7
4
)
 
2
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
4
9
5
)
 
 
4
.
4
4
*
 
(
2
.
2
0
)
 
2
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
4
9
3
)
 
2
.
6
3
***
 
(
.
5
2
2
)
 
3
.
0
9
***
  
(
.
8
0
1
)
 
3
.
5
3
***
  
(
1
.
0
5
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
0
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Af
rica
n 
A
m
er
ica
n
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
2
.
3
3
***
 
(
4
.
5
7
)
 
2
4
.
2
6
***
 
(
6
.
0
7
)
 
2
0
.
4
6
***
 
(
4
.
6
0
)
 
2
0
.
1
0
***
 
(
6
.
3
2
)
 
3
0
.
9
1
***
 
(
6
.
6
5
)
 
4
5
.
5
1
***
 
(
9
.
8
2
)
 
IE
 
2
6
.
1
3
***
 
(
6
.
5
3
)
 
2
7
.
4
1
***
 
(
6
.
9
5
)
 
3
2
.
8
4
***
 
(
9
.
4
4
)
 
2
6
.
1
3
***
  
(
6
.
5
1
)
 
2
3
.
6
0
**
 
(
7
.
8
1
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
Ro
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
 
118 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
     
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
4
***
 
(
.
1
6
6
)
 
1
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
1
7
6
)
 
.
9
6
***
 
(
.
2
6
3
)
 
.
9
2
4
***
 
(
.
2
7
2
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
4
7
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
5
8
)
 
 
.
6
9
***
 
(
.
5
8
7
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
1
***
 
(
.
2
3
1
)
 
.
7
6
**
  
(
.
2
7
4
)
 
.
7
4
**
 
(
.
2
6
2
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
8
9
***
 
(
.
4
1
1
)
 
2
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
3
7
9
)
 
2
.
5
8
***
 
(
.
5
5
1
)
 
2
.
5
5
***
 
(
.
5
9
1
)
 
4
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
7
2
5
)
 
4
.
2
4
***
 
(
.
7
6
6
)
 
 
2
.
8
7
***
 
(
.
5
8
7
)
 
4
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
7
7
7
)
 
4
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
7
4
5
)
 
4
.
2
0
***
  
(
1
.
0
2
)
 
3
.
9
9
***
 
(
.
9
9
4
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
3
3
***
 
(
.
6
0
6
)
 
4
.
3
9
***
 
(
.
6
0
5
)
 
4
.
6
1
***
 
(
.
9
6
6
)
 
4
.
5
6
***
 
(
1
.
0
3
)
 
7
.
0
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
3
3
***
 
(
1
.
3
5
)
 
 
4
.
6
0
***
 
(
.
9
4
1
)
 
7
.
4
5
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
6
.
6
5
***
 
(
1
.
2
0
)
 
5
.
0
6
***
  
(
1
.
1
6
)
 
4
.
8
1
***
  
(
1
.
1
4
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
ty
 
2
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
3
4
3
)
 
1
.
5
8
***
 
(
.
2
7
2
)
 
2
.
0
9
***
 
(
.
5
4
4
)
 
2
.
0
6
***
 
(
.
5
6
1
)
 
2
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
4
7
4
)
 
2
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
4
9
5
)
 
 
1
.
5
1
***
 
(
.
4
5
9
)
 
2
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
4
9
3
)
 
2
.
6
3
***
 
(
.
5
2
2
)
 
1
.
4
3
**
 
(
.
5
0
5
)
 
1
.
3
6
***
  
(
.
4
8
5
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
  
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
  
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
  
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
***
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
  
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
H
is
pa
nic
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
R
eg
r
ess
io
n
 
P
a
r
a
mete
r
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
2
.
3
3
***
 
(
4
.
5
7
)
 
-
1
8
.
4
1
 
(
1
1
.
6
3
)
 
2
0
.
4
6
***
 
(
4
.
6
0
)
 
-
1
4
.
1
5
 
(
9
.
4
1
)
 
3
0
.
9
1
***
 
(
6
.
6
5
)
 
-
2
0
.
8
7
 
(
1
6
.
4
4
)
 
 
-
2
7
.
7
1
 
(
1
9
.
7
8
)
 
2
7
.
4
1
***
 
(
6
.
9
5
)
 
6
.
0
9
 
(
1
2
.
2
9
)
 
2
6
.
1
3
***
  
(
6
.
5
1
)
 
1
1
.
7
5
 
(
2
5
.
4
6
)
 
F
a
cto
r
 
Lo
a
d
in
g
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
B
ed
tim
e 
 
 
 
R
o
u
ti
n
e
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
1
.
0
0
 
(
.
0
0
0
)
 
  
R
eg
u
lar
 
B
ed
tim
e
 
.
9
4
***
 
(
.
1
6
6
)
 
1
.
0
7
***
 
(
.
1
7
6
)
 
1
.
5
9
 
(
.
9
6
9
)
 
.
8
5
*
 
(
.
3
3
4
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
4
7
)
 
1
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
2
5
8
)
 
 
1
.
1
3
 
(
.
7
0
1
)
 
1
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
2
5
4
)
 
1
.
0
1
***
 
(
.
2
3
1
)
 
2
.
2
8
 
(
1
.
9
4
)
 
1
.
8
0
 
(
1
.
4
5
)
 
  
R
ea
d
in
g
 
R
o
u
ti
n
es
 
2
.
8
9
***
 
(
.
4
1
1
)
 
2
.
6
8
***
 
(
.
3
7
9
)
 
3
.
9
0
+
 
(
2
.
2
5
)
 
1
.
9
2
***
 
(
.
5
7
6
)
 
4
.
1
5
***
 
(
.
7
2
5
)
 
4
.
2
4
***
 
(
.
7
6
6
)
 
 
4
.
2
8
*
 
(
2
.
0
8
)
 
4
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
7
7
7
)
 
4
.
1
7
***
 
(
.
7
4
5
)
 
9
.
4
4
 
(
7
.
4
8
)
 
7
.
1
7
 
(
5
.
6
2
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
P
lay
 
4
.
3
3
***
 
(
.
6
0
6
)
 
4
.
3
9
***
 
(
.
6
0
5
)
 
7
.
8
8
+
 
(
4
.
5
6
)
 
3
.
9
4
***
 
(
1
.
1
8
)
 
7
.
0
4
***
 
(
1
.
2
5
)
 
7
.
3
3
***
 
(
1
.
3
5
)
 
 
7
.
3
2
*
 
(
3
.
6
6
)
 
 
7
.
4
5
***
 
(
1
.
3
8
)
 
6
.
6
5
***
 
(
1
.
2
0
)
 
1
9
.
4
6
 
(
1
5
.
7
0
)
 
1
4
.
6
8
 
(
1
1
.
7
7
)
 
  
P
ar
en
t
-
C
h
ild
 
Ou
ts
id
e 
A
ct
iv
i
t
y
 
2
.
2
2
***
 
(
.
3
4
3
)
 
1
.
5
8
***
 
(
.
2
7
2
)
 
1
.
6
9
 
(
1
.
4
0
)
 
.
8
8
 
 
(
.
5
6
6
)
 
2
.
2
0
***
 
(
.
4
7
4
)
 
2
.
2
3
***
 
(
.
4
9
5
)
 
 
2
.
0
4
 
(
1
.
2
9
)
 
2
.
2
6
***
 
(
.
4
9
3
)
 
2
.
6
3
***
 
(
.
5
2
2
)
 
8
.
4
7
 
(
6
.
8
8
)
 
6
.
4
1
 
(
5
.
1
5
)
 
Mea
n
s
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
1
4
m
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
4
+
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
 
-
.
0
3
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
-
.
0
5
***
 
(
.
0
1
5
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
4
)
 
-
.
0
3
+
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
-
.
0
3
+
 
(
.
0
2
0
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
2
4
m
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
0
4
 
(
.
0
0
8
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
+
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
0
9
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
  
R
o
u
ti
n
es 
-
3
6
m
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
***
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
2
)
 
-
.
0
4
***
 
(
.
0
1
1
)
 
-
.
0
2
*
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
1
 
(
.
0
1
0
)
 
-
.
0
2
 
(
.
0
1
3
)
 
Table
 17(c
ont.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup
 Mode
l for R
ace/
Ethn
icit
y and Cognit
ive Out
comes
 
 
***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < .05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 
 
119 
 
 
  
Table
 17(c
ont.)
 
Mul
ti-G
roup M
od
el 
for
 Race
/Et
hnic
ity a
nd
 Co
gn
itive
 Outc
ome
s 
 ***
p  
< .001 
 **
p  
< .01   
*p 
 < 
.05   +
p  
< .1
0  
 
 
 F
it S
ta
tis
tics
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
?
2
 
(
df
)
 
2
7
7
9
.
9
3
**
*
 
(
1
1
6
8
)
 
2
1
1
1
.
0
5
**
*
 
(
1
1
4
0
)
 
1
9
5
7
.
6
1
**
*
 
(
1
1
0
6
)
 
1
9
4
4
.
8
9
**
*
 
(
1
1
0
4
)
 
2
7
7
0
.
4
8
**
*
 
(
1
1
6
8
)
 
2
1
0
2
.
0
2
**
*
 
(
1
1
4
0
)
 
 
1
9
3
6
.
5
6
**
*
 
(
1
1
0
4
)
 
2
7
9
0
.
6
1
**
*
 
(
1
1
6
8
)
 
2
1
2
5
.
3
4
**
*
  
(
1
1
4
0
)
 
1
9
5
9
.
5
0
**
*
 
(
1
1
0
6
)
 
1
9
5
8
.
9
9
**
*
 
(
1
1
0
4
)
 
??
2
 
(
df
)
 
 
6
6
8
.
8
8
***
 
 
(
2
8
)
 
8
2
2
.
3
2
***
 
(
6
2
)
 
1
2
.
7
2
*
*
 
(
2
)
 
 
6
6
8
.
4
6
***
 
 
(
2
8
)
 
 
8
3
3
.
9
2
***
 
(
6
4
)
 
 
6
6
5
.
2
7
***
 
 
(
2
8
)
 
8
3
1
.
1
1
***
 
(
6
2
)
 
.
5
1
 
(
2
)
 
C
o
m
p
ar
is
o
n
 
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
1
 
Mo
d
el 
3
 
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
Mo
d
el 
5
 
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
9
 
Mo
d
el 
1
1
 
C
FI
 
.
7
4
 
.
8
5
 
.
8
6
 
.
8
7
 
.
7
5
 
.
8
5
 
 
.
8
7
 
.
7
4
 
.
8
4
 
.
8
6
 
.
8
6
 
R
MSE
A
 
(
C
I
)
 
.
0
5
8
 
(
.
0
5
6
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
1
)
 
.
0
4
6
 
(
.
0
4
3
 
to
 
.
0
4
9
)
 
.
0
4
4
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
7
)
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
7
)
 
058
 
(
.
0
5
5
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
1
)
 
.
0
4
6
 
(
.
0
4
3
 
to
 
.
0
4
9
)
 
 
.
0
4
3
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
6
)
 
059
 
(
.
0
5
6
 
to
 
 
.
0
6
1
)
 
.
0
4
6
 
(
.
0
4
3
 
to
 
.
0
4
9
)
 
.
0
4
4
 
(
.
0
4
0
 
to
 
.
0
4
7
)
 
.
0
4
4
 
(
.
0
4
1
 
to
 
.
0
4
7
)
 
 
 
120 
 
Appendix C 
Figures 
Figure 1. Hypothesized latent growth curve models for family routines and parenting 
 
 
  ?2i 
?3i 
?4i 
?5i 
Parenting 
36 Months 
?11 
?6i 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Routines  
14 Months 
?6 
Routines  
24 Months 
?7 
Routines  
36 Months 
?8 
Parenting 
14 Months 
?9 
Parenting 
24 Months 
?10 
Parenting 
Intercept 
?4 
Parenting 
Slope 
?5 
Routines 
Intercept 
?2 
Routines 
Slope 
?3 
?7i 
?8i 
?9i 
?10i 
?11i 
1 
1 
1 
-22 
-12 
0 
1 
1 
1 
0 
-12 
-22 
 
 
121 
 
Figure 2. Hypothesized Regression Model 
  
 
 
 
36 month 
Outcomes 
(?1) 
1 Y1 
Y2 
?1i 
Parenting 
36 Months 
?7 
?2i 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Routines  
14 Months 
?2 
Routines  
24 Months 
?3 
Routines  
36 Months 
?4 
Parenting 
14 Months 
?5 
Parenting 
24 Months 
?6 
?4i 
?5i 
?6i 
?7i 
?1 
?2 
?3 
?4 
?3i 
?5 
?6 
 
 
122 
 
Figure 3.  Fitted Path Diagram for Social Outcomes Measurement Model (standardized results 
with standard errors in parenthesis) 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 Social Outcomes 36-months  Emotion 
Regulation 
1 
.87*** 
(.230) 
-.16*** 
(.050) 
Orientation/ 
Engagement 
 .89 (.058) 
 .23 (.402) 
Aggressive 
Behavior 
 .98 (.016) 
 
 
123 
 
Figure 4. Fitted Path Diagram for Family Routines Measurement Model (standardized results 
with standard errors in parenthesis) 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Family 
Routines 
14-months  
Regular 
Bedtime 
1 
.78*** 
(.039) 
.22*** 
(.034) 
Bedtime 
Routine 
.94 
(.017) 
Parent-
Child Play .39 (.061
) 
Outside 
Activity 
.88 
(.026) 
Reading 
Routine 
 .58 
(.047) 
.35*** 
(.037) 
.65*** 
(.036) 
 
 
.95 
(.015)  
 
Regular 
Bedtime 
1 
.88*** 
(.039) 
.20*** 
(.032) 
Bedtime 
Routine 
.98 
(.010
) 
Parent-
Child Play .22 (.068) 
Outside 
Activity 
.89 
(.025
) 
Reading 
Routine 
 .52 
(.048) 
.33*** 
(.038) 
.69*** 
(.035) 
 
 
.96 
(.013
) 
 
 
Regular 
Bedtime 
1 
.83*** 
(.042) 
.14*** 
(.034) 
Bedtime 
Routine 
.98 
(.010) 
Parent-
Child Play .31 (.070) 
Outside 
Activity 
.87 
(.027
) 
Reading 
Routine 
 .58 
(.050) 
.36*** 
(.038) 
.65*** 
(.038) 
 
 
.98 
(.009
) 
 
 
 
Family 
Routines 
24-months  
 
Family 
Routines 
36-months  
 
 
 
.49*** 
(.035) 
.55** 
(.031) 
.49*** 
(.036) 
 
 
124 
 
Figure 5. Fitted Path Diagram for General Parenting Measurement Model (standardized results 
with standard errors in parenthesis) 
 
  
 Parenting 
24-months  Lang and Cog Dev. 
1 
.48*** 
(.038) 
-.35*** 
(.036) 
 Parenting 
36-months  Lang and Cog Dev. 
1 
.60*** 
(.035) 
-.46*** 
(.032) 
.96*** 
(.048) 
1.02 *** 
(.042) 
.93*** 
(.047)  .77 (.036) 
 
 
 
Support  .68 (.044) 
 Parenting 
14-months  
Lang and 
Cog Dev. 
1 
.46*** 
(.038) 
-.30*** 
(.037) 
Support  .62 (.052
) 
 .79 
(.035
) 
Discipline    .91 (.022
) 
Discipline     .88 (.025) 
Support  .72 (.039) 
 .65 
(.041) 
Discipline      .79 (.030) 
 
 
125 
 
Figure 6. Multi-group model for the relationship between family routines at 14, 24, or 36 months 
and child social competence at 36 months by child gender (girls are represented by orange and 
boys by maroon; standardized results for regression free model with standard errors in 
parenthesis) 
 
 
 
  
 
?
1i
 
 
 
1 
Y
1
 
Y
2
 
 Y
3
 
 14 Month 
Routine
s 
 
24  
Month 
Routine
s 
 36 Month 
Routines 
36 Month 
Social 
Competence 
.32
***
   .18
*
  
(.08)    (.08) 
.20
** 
  .27
***
 
  (.07)  (.07)             
.22
**
   .36
***
 
(.08)  (.08) 
Girls  Boys  
     
 
 
126 
 
Figure 7. Multi-group model for the relationship between family routines at 14, 24, or 36 months 
and child cognitive ability at 36 months by child gender (girls are represented by orange and 
boys by maroon; standardized results for regression free model with standard errors in 
parenthesis) 
 
  
 
?
1i
 
 
 
1 Y1 
Y
2
 
 14 Month 
Routine
s 
 
24  
Month 
Routine
s 
 36 Month 
Routines 
36 Month 
Cognitive 
Ability 
.24
***
   .21
***
  
  (.05)    (.05) 
.24
*** 
  .24
***
 
 (.04)    (.05)             
.16
***
   .26
***
 
  (.05)     (.05) 
Girls  Boys  
     
 
 
127 
 
Figure 8. Multi-group model for the relationship between family routines at 14, 24, or 36 months 
and child social competence at 36 months by race/ethnicity (European Americans are represented 
by orange, African Americans by maroon, and Hispanics by brown; standardized results for 
regression free model with standard errors in parenthesis) 
 
  
 
?
1i
 
 
 
1 Y1 
Y
2
 
 Y
3
 
 14 Month 
Routine
s 
 24 Month 
Routines 
 36  Month 
Routines 
36 Month 
Social 
Competence 
.20
**
   .39
**
   -.14  
(.07)    (.13)    (.17) 
European 
American   
African 
American 
Hispanic 
.17
**
   .36
**
   -.09 
(.06)    (.13)    (.16) 
.19
**
   .53
***
   .11 
(.07)    (.14)    (.15) 
 
 
128 
 
Figure 9. Multi-group model for the relationship between family routines at 14, 24, or 36 months 
and child cognitive ability at 36 months by race/ethnicity (European Americans are represented 
by orange, African Americans by maroon, and Hispanics by brown; standardized results for 
regression free model with standard errors in parenthsis) 
 
 
36  
Month 
Routines 
 14 Month 
Routines 
24 
Month 
Routines 
 
 
?
1i
 
 
 
1 Y
1
 
Y
2
 
36 Month 
Cognitive 
Ability 
.27
***
   .29
***
   -.20  
(.05)    (.06)    (.12) 
European 
American   
African American 
Hispanic 
     
.22
***
   .34
***
   -.14 
(.05)    (.05)    (.11) 
.22
***
   .26
***
   .05 
(.05)    (.06)    (.10)